Training the
brain or tending a garden? Students’ metaphors of learning
predict self-reported learning patterns
Elisabeth Wegner, Matthias Nückles
University of Freiburg, Germany
Article received 18 September
/ revised 30 November
/ accepted 6 December / available online 20
January
Abstract
Conceptions of learning are seen as an
important factor in shaping students’ patterns of learning.
However, conceptions are often implicit and difficult to
assess. Metaphors have been proposed as a method to assess
conceptions, because metaphors are closely linked to the
conceptual system. Therefore, in our study we assessed which
conceptions of learning are visible in students’ metaphors
of learning and examined whether these metaphors predict
differences in students’ learning patterns. Altogether, N =
91 students of educational science from a German university
filled in a questionnaire on their personal metaphors of
learning, their learning strategy use, epistemological
beliefs, and their motivation. Four kinds of metaphors could
be differentiated: regulation-related metaphors, learning as
knowledge acquisition, learning as problem solving, or as
personality development. A discriminant analysis revealed
that students with personality development metaphors and
with problem solving metaphors were more intrinsically
motivated and more aware of the relativism of knowledge than
students with regulation-related or knowledge acquisition
metaphors. Students with personality development metaphors
differed from students with problem solving metaphors in
their stronger use of deep processing strategies, their
lower extrinsic motivation and their stronger rejection of a
dualism of knowledge. The study demonstrates that metaphors
of learning are a suitable tool for assessing students’
conceptions of learning and gives new insights on using this
innovative method as an assessment tool.
Keywords: conceptions of learning; metaphors; learning
patterns; approaches to learning
“Learning is like
rowing against the current. As soon as you stop, you drift
back again.” Benjamin
Britten (1913-76)
“The roots of education are
bitter, but the fruit is sweet” (Aristotle, 384 -382 b.c.)
1.
Introduction
A great number of proverbs tell us
in in metaphors what learning is like, how learning occurs,
and what the benefits of learning are. Such metaphorical
expressions have received a lot of attention from researchers
from as diverse domains as philosophy (Black, 1993), cognitive
science (Gick & Holyoak, 1980) or cognitive linguistics
(Lakoff & Johnson, 1980), because metaphors have been
identified as being more than a deviation from the ‘normal
use’ of language. Instead, metaphors are closely linked to the
way our conceptual system is structured, thus being one of the
basic mechanisms in which we perceive the world (Lakoff &
Johnson, 1980). In the context of cognitively oriented
research, conceptual metaphors are usually defined as a
situation or an object X that shares a similarity with a
situation or an object Y (“X is like Y”). The situation or
object X that is characterized by the metaphor is called the
“target”, and the situation or object Y that is the medium of
comparison, the “source” of the metaphor. Because conceptual
metaphors are based on the detection of similarities of new
experiences with familiar experiences, they help to understand
novel information, concepts, or information (Gentner &
Holyoak, 1997, p. 32). For example, Britten’s metaphor of
learning as rowing against the current helps to convey the
importance of learning continuously. However, metaphors only partially structure
an experience, because the target and source of a metaphor
never match completely. Obviously, the rowing metaphor leaves
out important other aspects of learning, such as that learning
produces positive outcomes, as in Aristotle’s metaphor of
education, or that learning requires the learner to link new
information to existing knowledge, which becomes visible in a
metaphor such as “Learning is like weaving a net”. According
to Lakoff and Johnson’s conceptual metaphor theory, the
metaphors that are used also feed back into our conceptual
systems. For example, the metaphors “TIME IS A RESOURCE” and
“WORK IS A RESOURCE” bring us to the realisation that leisure
time is also a resource, thus influencing our concepts of
leisure to be perceived as a valuable good that must not be
wasted (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980). Thus metaphors act as a
lens through which we perceive the world around us. Landau,
Meier
and Keefer (2010) suggest
that metaphors
are so fundamental for human thinking, that in order to
understand individuals’ actions with regard to abstract social
concepts, such as justice, spirituality, or happiness, it is
central to look at how individuals structure these concepts
metaphorically:
“…metaphor is a cognitive tool that people
routinely use to interpret and evaluate information related to
those abstract concepts. Put simply, a metaphor-enriched
perspective suggests that a complete account of the meanings
people give to abstract, socially relevant concepts requires
an understanding not only of their schematic knowledge about
those concepts in isolation but also how they structure those
concepts in terms of superficially dissimilar, relatively more
concrete concepts.” (p.1047)
Therefore, we assume that metaphors
could be an important tool to assess how students structure
their concepts of learning. The aim of the current study was
therefore to assess which kind of metaphors students use to
describe learning and which impact the metaphors have on
students’ learning. So far, there is only very little research
assessing students’ metaphors of learning. However, we find
ample research on students’ conceptions of
learning. Therefore, we will first outline findings on
conceptions of learning and their role for how students learn.
Afterwards we will elaborate on how metaphors and conceptions
might relate to each other and how metaphors have been used to
assess conceptions. Finally we will present evidence from our
study indicating that indeed the metaphors that students use
relate to their self-reported learning activities, their
motivation and their epistemological beliefs, that is, their
beliefs about knowledge and knowing.
1.1
Conceptions of learning
Conceptions
can be defined as an “individual’s personal and therefore
variable response to a concept” (Entwistle & Peterson,
2004, p. 408). Conceptions are usually understood as systems
of beliefs (e.g. Marton & Säljö, 1976; Richardson, 2007),
which act as a filter for cognition (see Pajares, 1992). The
kind of conception of learning a student holds organizes the
student's perception of learning environments, the
interpretation of learning tasks, the expectations towards
teaching staff and other students, motivation and also the
choice of learning strategies (Pajares, 1992). Early studies
on students’ conceptions of learning (Säljö, 1979)
differentiated between five different conceptions, ranging
from reproductive conceptions such as understanding learning
as the acquisition of factual information and as memorizing
what has been learned, over learning as the application and
use of knowledge, to meaning oriented conceptions such as
understanding what has been learned and as seeing things in a
different way. According to the phenomenographic perspective,
conceptions
are understood as qualitatively distinct categories, but
“higher” conceptions such as “developing as a person” subsume
lower conceptions, such as “acquisition of knowledge”.
Individuals develop towards more advanced conceptions (e.g.
Marton & Säljö, 1976). Other researchers do not
assume a developmental order of distinct and developmental
categories of conceptions (e,g. Richardson, 2007).
Later
research focused on how students’ understanding of learning is
related to students’ use of learning strategies, their
learning motivation and their epistemological beliefs. In this
productive area of research, two merging research
frameworks can be discerned (Vanthournout, Donche, Gijbels &
van Petegem, 2014), namely the learning patterns framework (Vermunt, 1996;
Vermunt & Vermetten, 2004) and the approaches to learning
framework (e.g. Entwistle & Peterson, 2004; Entwistle &
Ramsden, 1983). Both frameworks are based on the assumption that
there are, on the one hand, different dimensions of learning on
which students individually vary (such as their use of learning
strategies, their learning motivation or their self-regulation
strategies), but that on the other hand, these dimensions form
systematic clusters, which are called learning patterns or
approaches to learning. Richardson (2011) assumes that students’
conceptions of learning are important for forming these
systematic clusters.
Interestingly, in both research
frameworks, we find a pattern that is characterized by an
intrinsic interest in studying and in learning contents (deep approach /
meaning-directed learning pattern). Students with this
pattern use deep processing strategies and have a high level
of self-regulation, and according to Vermunt (1996), this
pattern is characterized by a mental model of learning as
construction of knowledge. Also, both frameworks describe an
opposing pattern in which students have the major intention to
cope with course requirements, are externally motivated, see
contents as unrelated bits of knowledge and fail to see the
meaning or value of the contents. This goes in hand with
learning strategies that focus on rehearsal and involve little
reflection, and also with a feeling of pressure and anxiety (surface approach /
reproduction-directed learning pattern). This pattern is
based on the mental model of learning as intake of knowledge
(Vermunt, 1996).
Both frameworks also
describe, apart from these two more or less identical types of
students, additional patterns or approaches. Within the
learning patterns framework, Vermunt (1996) describes a type
of students with an undirected
learning pattern. This pattern is characterized by a
lack of regulation, ambivalent motivation, and no identifiable
mental model of learning. The other type of student described
by Vermunt (1996) are those with an application directed
learning pattern, which is based on the mental model of
learning as the use of knowledge, an intrinsic (vocational)
orientation and concrete processing of information. The
approaches to learning framework additionally includes a strategic approach
(Biggs, 1987; Entwistle, Tait, & McCune, 2000). This
approach is characterized by a strong motivation to do well in
the course and to complete the degree in order to accomplish
personal goals. Students with a strategic approach organize
their studying well, manage their time effectively, and are
alert to assessment requirements and criteria (Virtanen &
Lindblom-Ylänne, 2010). In a comprehensive review, Vermunt and
Vermetten (2004) found that an undirected pattern/surface
approach leads to the worst studying results; the best
studying results are yielded by the meaning-directed
pattern/deep approach. Reproduction-directed pattern and
application-directed pattern had no clear relation to studying
success.
1.2
Assessing conceptions of learning
Taken together, we can draw from
research that conceptions play an important role in shaping
students’ learning, and thus have an impact on their studying.
However, assessment of conceptions is not as simple as it
seems. As we have pointed out above, conceptions are partly
implicit and therefore difficult to assess. Interviews which
could be used to assess also implicit aspects of conceptions
are time consuming and are not suitable for large scale
studies. Often, questionnaires with
dimensional assessment scales such as the Inventory of
Learning Styles (ILS, Vermunt, 1994) are used to assign
students to distinct groups by using cluster analysis (e.g.
Parpala, Lindblom-Ylänne, Komulainen, Litmanen, & Hirsto,
2010; Entwistle & McCune, 2013; Richardson, 2007).
However, the technique of cluster analysis carries the risk of
methodological artefacts, because general answer tendencies
might account for correlations between two variables
(Richardson, 2011). For example, some persons tend to agree
rather than to disagree on items (acquiescent response style),
whereas others tend to choose extreme response categories on
all scales. This can result in clusters not based on
differences in the assessed dimensions, but on the general
answer tendencies. Another problem is that clusters can only
be determined post hoc in large samples, but it is difficult
to make an individual diagnosis of conceptions. Consequently,
assessment
techniques are needed to determine conceptions of
learning. Given the important role of metaphors for our
cognitive system, it comes as no surprise that recently in
the area of teacher education and of higher education in
general, metaphors have become increasingly popular for
assessing implicit constructs such as conceptions (Löfström,
Nevgi, Wegner, & Karm, 2015).
1.3
Using metaphors for understanding conceptions
To use metaphors to assess
conceptions of learning, we need to take a closer look into
how metaphors and conceptions are assumed to relate to each
other, and how this has been exploited in research.
Unfortunately, educational researchers using metaphors often
do not explicate which relation between metaphors and
conceptions they assume. This is problematic because Lakoff’s
and Johnson’s cognitive metaphor theory, which is still the
most prominent metaphor theory, allows for different
assumptions about the relation between metaphors and
cognition. Murphy (1996) describes a “strong version” of this
theory, stating “that some concepts are not understood via
their own representations but instead by (metaphoric)
reference to a different domain” (p. 201). This would imply
that a metaphor of learning is identical to a conception of
learning. A person who describes learning as the construction
of a skyscraper would then literally have the conception that
learning is construction. In contrast, the “weak version” of
cognitive metaphor theory assumes that both the source and the
target concept of the metaphor are more or less developed
separate cognitive structures. Under this view, a certain
conception is the reason why a person can identify features
that are mappings between one's own conception and a certain
metaphor (Haser, 2005). Thus, a person who has the conception
of learning as a construction of knowledge would single out
identical features between learning and building a skyscraper,
but not between learning and eating, and thus prefer to use
the metaphor of learning as building a skyscraper then as
eating a cake as a descriptor.
In research using metaphors for
assessing conceptions we can find works based on the “strong”
and the “weak" versions of cognitive metaphor theory. Those
researchers who are interested in examining the development
and change of conceptions, for example in the context of
educational development programs (e.g. Bullough, 1991;
Clandinin, 1985), tend to argue on the base of a strong
version of cognitive metaphor theory because they usually
assume that changing the metaphor a person uses also leads to
a change in the person’s conception. In contrast, researchers
using metaphors mainly for assessment of conceptions (e.g.
Saban, Kocbeker & Saban, 2007; Patchen & Crawford,
2011) usually argue on the base of a weak version of the
cognitive metaphor theory, assuming that metaphors help to
express or to identify an underlying conception. Based on the
longstanding tradition on research on conceptions of teaching
and learning (e.g. Gow & Kember, 1993; Vermunt &
Vermetten, 2004), we assume that there are indeed underlying
conceptions that are separate from metaphors, and thus would
adhere to a weak version of cognitive metaphor theory. We
assume that the underlying conception enables or prompts a
person to identify structural mappings between one's own
conception and the metaphor.
Two principally different approaches
can be discerned in assessing conceptions via metaphors
(Löfström et. al., 2015). On the one hand, researchers
themselves generate metaphors and use them as a stimulus for
assessing conceptions. For example, some researchers have
developed questionnaires in which participants are asked to
rate metaphors (e.g., Lehmann, 2012). Others have asked
participants to reflect on preselected written metaphors
(e.g., Visser-Wijnween, van Driel, van der Rijst, Verloop
& Visser, 2009) or metaphorical pictures (Ben-Peretz,
Mendelson & Kron, 2003), and analysed the participants’
responses with regard to the underlying conception. In both
cases, the participants mapped preselected metaphors to their
own conception. On the other hand, researchers also asked
participants to produce metaphors on their own, and then
analysed these metaphors according to their conceptual
content. For example, Saban et al. (2007) asked more than 1000
students to write down a metaphor on being a teacher and
identified six dominant conceptual mappings for the metaphors:
knowledge provider, craftsperson, facilitator, nurturer,
counsellor and democratic leader. Interestingly, these
conceptual categories are similar to conceptions of teaching
as described in the “teaching perspectives inventory” by
Collins and Pratt (2011), namely, transmission,
apprenticeship, developmental, nurturing and social reform.
Other studies (Patchen & Crawford, 2011; Wegner &
Nückles, 2015a) classified metaphors based on the two
scientific paradigms of learning as acquisition vs. as
participation according to Sfard (1998). Only a few studies
focus on students’ metaphors. Inbar (1996) asked more than 400
students for metaphors on ‘being a student’ and on ‘teachers’.
A great proportion of metaphors was related to feeling
imprisoned in school, showing largely negative emotions
towards school. Marsch (2009) analysed high school students’
metaphors of biology learning. She found that most students
conveyed an idea of learning as intake of knowledge. In a
longitudinal study with students from educational science,
Wegner and Nückles (2015b) found that students adapted their
metaphors of learning to university learning culture in the
course of their first year of studying. While in the first
year, the most frequently used metaphor of learning was
“collecting”, the most frequently used metaphor in the second
year described learning as “discovering”.
Even though empirical studies do
indicate that different views on teaching or learning are
visible in metaphors, and there are theoretical arguments for
a close relationship between metaphors and conceptions, there
are few studies which really validate whether different
metaphors also account for differences in underlying
conceptions, and even less, whether they also account for
differences in actual practice. Moreover, all of the existing
validation studies are case studies with very small samples,
or just report data on selected cases illustrating their
hypotheses (e.g. Mahlios, Massengill‐Shaw, & Barry, 2010; Bullough, 1991;
Marsch, 2009; Thomas & McRobbie, 1999). Some larger studies
link metaphors of teaching to other self-reported data, but not
to practice (Wegner & Nückles, 2015a; Löfström &
Poom-Valickis, 2013). Thus, there is a need for empirical
studies validating whether metaphors of learning can indeed be
an indicator for conceptions of learning, and whether students’
metaphors of learning really relate to how students learn in
terms of which learning strategies they use and what their
motivation is.
1.4
Summary and aims of the study
In sum, we can conclude that
students’ learning patterns are influenced by the individual
understanding by students of what learning is, that is, their
conceptions of learning. First evidence from studies on
conceptions of teaching indicates that metaphors might also be
an appropriate and helpful tool for assessing conceptions of
learning, and that differences in metaphors of learning are
also associated with differences in students’ learning
practice. However, so far there are only few studies analysing
the relation between metaphors and practice, and studies are
only based on small sample sizes. In our study, we aimed
at closing these gaps by (a) exploring whether
the different conceptions of learning as they have been
described in the literature are also visible in the metaphors
that students use to describe learning, and (b) examining
whether differences in the conceptual content of the metaphors
account for differences in learning practice, such as the use
of learning strategies, study motivation, and epistemological
beliefs.
2. Methods
2.1
Participants and procedure
Ninety-one students of Educational
Science from a German university took part in the study (78.1%
female and 21.9% male, meanage =23.81 years, SDage
=3.38). All students were first given a short example of what
we meant by metaphor, and were then asked to write down their
metaphors of learning. Afterwards they filled-in
questionnaires on learning-related measures. All measurements
took place in university courses in the Institute of
Educational Science and were set at the beginning of a lesson.
2.2
Questionnaires
For assessing learning-related
measures, we chose questionnaires on motivation, learning
strategies and epistemological beliefs which are well
established for German language speakers and which address
central aspects of learning patterns and approaches to
learning (for an overview of the scales and their
reliabilities, see Table 1).
Table 1
Scales of the questionnaires, scale
reliability (Cronbach’s ɑ), mean values (M), standard
deviation (SD) and number of items.
|
Sample
item |
ɑ |
M |
SD |
No.
of items |
Intrinsic
motivation |
I
don’t need a reward for completing the study tasks
because they are fun. |
.
734 |
4.96 |
0.91 |
5 |
Extrinsic
motivation |
I
will be quite proud when I have completed my degree. |
.726 |
5.82 |
0.96 |
3 |
Organisation |
I
draw tables and graphs in order to structure the
contents of the subject. |
.791 |
3.68 |
0.61 |
8 |
Elaboration |
I
try to relate new concepts or theories to familiar
concepts or theories. |
.742 |
3.67 |
0.55 |
8 |
Critical
thinking |
I
examine whether theories, interpretations or
conclusions are sufficiently grounded. |
.873 |
3.11 |
0.68 |
8 |
Rehearsal |
I
re-read my notes again and again. |
.827 |
3.14 |
0.75 |
7 |
Metacognitive
strategies |
Before
I start with learning, I try to plan which contents I
do need to know and which I don’t. |
.745 |
3.59 |
0.47 |
11 |
Time
management |
I
schedule time slots for studying. |
.897 |
2.99 |
0.95 |
5 |
Learning
with others |
I
work on texts and tasks together with my colleagues. |
.849 |
3.41 |
0.77 |
7 |
Relativism |
Scientific
research shows that there is one right answer to most
problems. |
.635 |
1.69 |
.40 |
6 |
Dualism |
If
two scientists have a different opinion on a matter,
one of them has to be wrong. |
.613 |
1.68 |
.43 |
4 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
The use of learning strategies was
assessed by seven scales of a German questionnaire (LIST; Wild
& Schiefele, 1994) which is based on the Motivated
Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich, Smith,
Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993). Participants rated on a
five-point rating scale how often they engage in certain
learning activities (“In
the following, we would like to know about how you learn. You
will find a list of learning activities. Please indicate for
each activity,
how often it occurs when you are learning. You can rate the
frequency between very seldom (1) and very often (5)”).
The selected activities addressed four cognitive strategies
(organization of contents, elaboration of contents, rehearsal,
critical thinking), metacognitive strategies, use of time
management strategies and the frequencies of learning with
others.
Motivational orientation was
assessed by two scales of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory
(IMI; Deci & Ryan, 2003), one on intrinsic motivation and
one on the extrinsic value of studying in a version adapted to
the context of higher education. Students were instructed to
rate the items on their seven-point rating scale ranging from
completely disagree
to completely agree (“Please indicate for each
statement how much you agree. […] These questionnaires are
not evaluated! There are no “right” or “wrong” answers.”)
Epistemological beliefs in general
were assessed by a German questionnaire on epistemological
beliefs (Köller, Watermann, Trautwein, & Lüdtke, 2004). It
comprises two dimensions, “dualism” (sample item: “If two
scientists have a different opinion on a matter, one of them
has to be wrong.”) and “relativism” (sample item: “Scientific
insights that seem true today can turn out to be wrong”).
Participants had to rate the statements on a four-point rating
scale ranging from totally disagree (= 1) to totally agree (=
4).
2.3
Assessment and analysis of metaphors
Following Saban (Saban et al.,
2007), students had to answer the questions “Learning is like…
because…”. In order to enrich the answers, we added the
question “The goal of learning is…”. Metaphors were analysed
following Chi’s recommendations on coding verbal data (1997).
Two metaphors were excluded from the analysis because they
were only fragments. One metaphor as a whole was defined as
the unit of analysis, that is, the complete answer consisting
of the source and explanation of the metaphor, because
sometimes the same source was associated with different kinds
of explanations (e.g. “learning is like food: you need it for
survival” vs. "Learning is like eating food: if you eat too
much, you get sick”). We then inductively developed a system
of categories within a team of two researchers. All decisions
were also discussed within a larger research team, consisting
of four researchers in total. As in other studies (e.g. Inbar,
1996; Leavy, McSorley & Boté, 2007) , we found a large
amount of metaphors without conceptual content, but merely
related to aspects of regulating one’s own learning and
motivation, such as in "Learning is like jumping into cold
water. Usually you don’t want to do it, but once you get
started, it’s always good”. Therefore, regulation-related
metaphors were first separated from other metaphors. In the
second step, the remaining metaphors were classified according
to the conceptual content. We distinguished three different
kinds of metaphors: learning as acquisition of knowledge vs.
learning as problem solving vs. learning as development of
personality (see Table 2). For each category, a short
description was written down with examples. Then,half of the
metaphors (N=43) were coded by a second independent person.
Interrater-reliability as measured by Cohen’s κ was very good
(κ = .81).
3. Results
Conceptions of learning as described
in literature were visible in our metaphors. Of the four
categories of metaphors, knowledge acquisition was the most
common (30.3%), followed closely by regulation-related
metaphors (28.1%). Personality development metaphors were
described by 25.8% of the students, and only 15.7% of the
students used metaphors which focused on learning as a
prerequisite for solving problems (Table 3).
Table 2
Categories of metaphors, description and anchoring
examples for each category of metaphor
Category |
Description |
Example |
N |
Regulation-related
metaphors |
The
metaphor and its explanation refer to self-regulation
aspects and do not contain any information about
cognitive processes or further goals in learning. |
“Learning
is like jumping into cold water. Usually you don’t
want to do it, but once you get started, it’s always
good.” “Learning
is like climbing a mountain. Some hills are steep, and
others are easy to walk.” |
25 (28.1%) |
Acquisition
of knowledge |
Learning
consists of the acquisition of something (=knowledge).
There is no further indication that the acquired
knowledge is used for something. |
“Learning
is like building a library with your own books. You
start with one shelf and while you get more and more
books you also need more shelves.” “Learning
is like solving a jigsaw puzzle … the goal is to solve
the jigsaw puzzle and to get the complete picture.” |
27
(30.3%) |
Problem
solving |
Learning
consists of the acquisition of something (= skills and
knowledge) which are necessary to solve certain
problems, to be prepared for future challenges or to
be able to work in a certain job. |
„Learning
is like food – you need it for survival. Without it
you cannot deal with new problems.” “Learning
is like getting a closet with lots of clothes. At the
beginning of your life you have only a few pieces of
clothes, later you get more and more […]. The goal is
to buy, to select, to sort, to categorize the clothes
so you can use them and wear them when you need them.”
|
14 (15.7%) |
Development
of personality |
Learning
consists of developing something existing further, in
order to develop one's own personality or new
perspectives. |
“Learning
is like exploring other countries. You get to know new
cultures and new perspectives, and you widen your
horizon.” “Learning
is like a plant that is growing, because you thrive
and prosper inside.” |
23 (25.8%) |
Total |
|
|
89 (100%) |
In the next step, we determined
whether students with different kinds of metaphors differed
with regards to their epistemological beliefs, their study
motivation, and their learning strategies. An overall MANOVA
with type of metaphor as independent measure, and
epistemological beliefs, motivation and learning strategies as
dependent measures showed a significant multivariate effect of
metaphor type, F(33,
231) = 2.31, p <
.001, η2=.25 (see Table 3 for an overview of the
descriptive data for the four kinds of metaphors). Separate
univariate ANOVAs revealed significant differences for
intrinsic motivation, F(3,
85) = 4.31, p <
.01, η2=.13,
for dualism F(3,85)
= 2.78, p < .05,
η2=.09, and the use of rehearsal strategies, F(3,85) = 4.31, p < .01, η2=
.14. Students with problem solving and development metaphors
indicated a higher intrinsic motivation than students with
regulation-related or knowledge acquisition metaphors (see
Table 3). Students with personality development metaphors had
the lowest scores on the dualism scale, while students with
knowledge acquisition metaphors had the highest, indicating
that students with knowledge acquisition metaphors believed
much stronger that knowledge is either true or false than
students with personality development metaphors. Students with
knowledge acquisition metaphors also had the strongest
tendency to use rehearsal strategies, followed by students
with regulation-related and personality development metaphors.
Students with problem-solving metaphors had the lowest scores
on this scale.
Table 3
Means and standard deviation for study motivation,
epistemological beliefs and learning strategies for each
group of metaphors
|
Regulation-related |
Acquisition
of knowledge |
Problem
solving |
Development
of personality |
Intrinsic
motivation |
4.70
(0.76) |
4.66
(0.98) |
5.36
(0.90) |
5.33
(0.81) |
Extrinsic
motivation |
5.70
(1.11) |
5.91
(0.89) |
6.11
(0.66) |
5.70
(1.03) |
Relativism |
1.77
(0.42) |
1.80
(0.44) |
1.63
(0.42) |
1.52
(0.29) |
Dualism |
1.66
(0.37) |
1.82
(0.46) |
1.75
(0.38) |
1.49
(0.43) |
Critical
thinking |
3.06
(0.75) |
3.01
(0.58) |
3.01
(0.71) |
3.35
(0.71) |
Learning
with others |
3.42
(0.68) |
3.50
(0.86) |
3.00
(0.62) |
3.58
(0.80) |
Elaboration |
3.70
(0.53) |
3.69
(0.54) |
3.57
(0.48) |
3.68
(0.65) |
Organisation |
3.53
(0.37) |
3.80
(0.58) |
3.68
(0.83) |
3.71
(0.71) |
Rehearsal
|
3.05
(0.75) |
3.52
(0.57) |
2.70
(0.82) |
3.06
(0.75) |
Metacognitive
strategies |
3.57
(0.43) |
3.75
(0.55) |
3.43
(0.46) |
3.52
(0.42) |
Time
management |
2.73
(0.78) |
3.29
(0.95) |
3.30
(0.92) |
2.75
(1.06) |
To better understand the overall
differences between the groups, and the patterns of
motivation, epistemology and learning strategies for each
group, we performed a discriminant analysis with
epistemological beliefs, motivation, and learning strategies
as predictors and the kind of metaphors as criterion. It
resulted in three discriminant functions. The first
discriminant function explained half of the variance, 56.8%,
canonical R2 = .38; the second discriminant
function explained one third of the variance, 33.2% canonical
R2 = .26. The third discriminant function explained
the remaining 9.9% of the variance, canonical R2 =
.09. Together, the three functions significantly
differentiated between the metaphor types (Wilk’s Λ = .41, χ2(33)
= 71.89, p = .000). After
removing the first function, the remaining two functions still
contributed significantly to the classification of the
metaphors (Wilk’s Λ = .66, χ2(20) = 33.12, p = .03). However,
the last function on its own could not differentiate between
the metaphors. Figure 1 shows the distribution of the four
metaphors among the two separating functions. Correlations of
the predicting variables with each canonical discriminant
function are given in Table 4.
A closer look at the discriminant
functions revealed that the first function separated the
students with regulation-related and with knowledge
acquisition metaphors from the students with personality
development and problem solving metaphors, whereas the second
function mainly separated the students with problem solving
metaphors from the students with personality development
metaphors, see Fig. 1. The first function was associated with
high beliefs in the certainty of knowledge (i.e., low
relativism), and with a low intrinsic motivation, see Table 4
and Fig. 2), thus indicating that students with knowledge
acquisition and regulation-related metaphors were less
intrinsically motivated and believed to a higher extent that
knowledge is certain and unambiguous. The second function
correlated positively with extrinsic motivation and the belief
in the dualism of knowledge, and negatively with an extra
preference for critical thinking, for learning with other
students and for elaboration of contents (see Table 4). This
indicates that students with problem solving metaphors were
more extrinsically motivated, believed more that knowledge was
either wrong or right, and were less inclined to critically
think about the contents or to discuss them with colleagues,
than students with personality development metaphors.
Figure
1.
Plot of the group centroids of the four metaphors
with regard to the two discriminant functions. Function 1
separates problem
solving and personality development metaphors from the
regulation-related and
knowledge acquisition metaphors. Function 2 separates
personality development
from the problem solving metaphors. (see pdf)
Table 4
Correlations
between discriminant functions and the predicting variables.
Bold print
indicates the highest correlating function for each
predictor variable
|
Function 1:
Instrinsic motivation (-) and
variability of knowledge |
Function 2: Extrinsic motivation Deep processing (-), dualism |
Function 3: Structured learning |
Instrinsic
motivation |
-.467 |
-.144 |
.191 |
Relativism
(general certainty beliefs) |
.299 |
.250 |
-.198 |
Dualism
(beliefs in simple knowledge) |
.179 |
.460 |
.140 |
Learning
with others |
.172 |
-.316 |
.263 |
Critical
thinking |
-.107 |
-.309 |
.140 |
External
motivation |
-.068 |
.253 |
.149 |
Elaboration |
.062 |
-.097 |
-.078 |
Rehearsal |
.424 |
-.003 |
.702 |
Organisation |
-.008 |
.051 |
.475 |
Time
management |
-.002 |
.421 |
.461 |
Metacognitive
strategies |
.269 |
.060 |
.357 |
The
last canonical discriminant function helped to differentiate
the four groups
only together with the second function. On this function, we
found high
loadings of measures indicating structured learning, such as
the strategies of
organization and rehearsal, metacognitive strategies and time
management (see
Fig. 2). The function differentiated between
regulation-related metaphors and
knowledge acquisition metaphors, with students with knowledge
acquisition
metaphors showing more use of structured learning than
students with
regulation-related metaphors, that is, students with metaphors
which just focus
on aspects relating to the regulation of their learning or
their motivation
rather than on the results or the process of learning.
However, as noted above,
the third function could not discriminate between the groups
on its own.
Figure
2. z-standardized mean values for each of the
metaphor categories. The variables of the first function are
printed in
black/bold (discriminating between the regulation-related and
knowledge
acquisition metaphors on the one hand, and the problem-solving
and the
personality development metaphors on the other hand).
Variables of the second
function are given in hatched/italics (discriminating between
problem solving
metaphors and personality development metaphors). (see pdf)
4.
Discussion
and Conclusion
In
our study, we could distinguish four kinds of metaphors of
learning, namely
metaphors focusing on regulation aspects of learning,
metaphors expressing the
idea of learning as knowledge acquisition and the idea of
learning as
personality development, and metaphors focusing on learning as
a prerequisite
for problem solving. Students’ metaphors of learning predicted
different
patterns of motivation, epistemology and use of learning
strategies. Students
with problem solving and with personality development
metaphors differed in
their intrinsic motivation and their awareness for the
tentativeness of
knowledge from students with knowledge acquisition and with
regulation-related
metaphors. Students with personality metaphors could be
separated from students
with problem solving metaphors by their use of deep processing
strategies,
their belief in the dualism of knowledge and their extrinsic
motivation.
Finally, students with knowledge acquisition metaphors had a
tendency to engage
more in structured learning activities than students with
regulation-related
metaphors, though not significantly so.
Metaphors
of learning predicted study motivation, epistemological
beliefs and learning
strategies. This implies that metaphors can be used to detect
differences in conceptions
of learning. The predicted learning patterns mirror in some
respects both the
learning patterns and the approaches to learning model.
Personality development
metaphors seem to predict a meaning-directed learning pattern
or a deep
approach, because students with personality development
metaphors displayed a
high intrinsic study motivation, a high awareness for the
tentativeness and the
complexity of knowledge and indicated to make much use of deep
processing
strategies. This finding confirms results from Entwistle and
McCune (2013), who
found that there is a certain group of students that have a
‘disposition to
understand for oneself’. This disposition seems to be based on
the view of
learning as development of personality.
Students
with problem solving metaphors have similarities with students
with an
application-directed learning pattern as described by Vermunt
(1996), because
the application directed mental model of learning is based on
the use of
knowledge as well. Problem-solving metaphors were also
associated with strong
extrinsic motivation for studying, but were, other than
students with the
application-directed learning pattern, also more intrinsically
motivated. On
the other hand, students with problem-solving metaphors made
only average use
of concrete processing strategies such as elaboration of
contents, which would
have been expected in an application-directed learning
pattern.
Knowledge
acquisition metaphors seem to be similar to Vermunts’
rehearsal-directed
learning pattern, because they are also characterized by a
mental model of
intake of knowledge. As students with a rehearsal-directed
learning approach,
students with knowledge acquisition metaphors had an extrinsic
study motivation
and believed in the stability of knowledge. However, other
than students with a
rehearsal-directed learning pattern, students with knowledge
acquisition
metaphors in our sample also described the use of deep
learning strategies and
structured their learning activities strongly. In this
respect, they seem more
similar to the strategic approach described by Biggs (1987),
which is
characterized by good organization, good time management, and
alertness to the
assessment requirements and criteria. This would indicate that
acquisition and
elaboration of knowledge are seen as the dominant requirement
within the degree
under consideration.
Finally,
regulation-related metaphors have a great overlap with the
undirected learning
pattern. The metaphors did not convey a mental model of
learning, students had
little intrinsic motivation and they did not engage in deep or
structured
learning activities. This is interesting in several respects.
On the one hand,
these findings mirror those of other studies in which
participants described
metaphors with no apparent match to conceptions of teaching or
learning. For
example, in Inbar's (1996) study with high school students,
most metaphors were
related to emotional aspects of learning and did not reveal
anything about
underlying conceptions of learning. Similarly, in their study
on teacher
candidates’ metaphors of teaching, Leavy et al. (2007) report
a great number of
metaphors that did “not refer to components central to the
practice of
teaching, but referred to what teaching meant to the
individuals themselves
(e.g. ‘teaching is like running a marathon; you train, sweat,
and prepare for
this great race but once you’re in it, you just keep going
strong until the
end’)” (p. 1226). In
one group of the
sample, 30% of the metaphors were ‘self-referential’. Such
self-referential metaphors
can be found in many studies using metaphors (e.g. Leavy et.
al., 2007; Zapata
& Lacorte, 2007; Löfström & Poom-Valickis, 2013).
Findings from our
study might be a first indicator that the participants who use
such
self-referential, emotional or motivational metaphors have not
yet developed a
differentiated explicable conception which can be communicated
by a metaphor.
Considering the unorganized use of learning strategies in this
group, the
finding could be interpreted in the way that a lack of an
elaborated conception
of learning is a major problem for developing adequate
learning strategies.
Consequently, to these students challenges of self-regulation
are the most
distinct experience of learning. However, further research is
needed to confirm
this hypothesis.
Of course, some limitations have to be
born in mind.
Again, our study only assessed self-report data on
participants’ use of
learning strategies in general. Therefore, we do not know how
students’ answers
relate to their actual practice of learning or on what they think they should do.
Also, course
requirements, which influence strongly how students actually
learn, need to be
considered (Vermetten, Lodewijks & Vermunt, 1999).
However, if students
were biased in their answers on their learning strategy use,
the differences in
self-report data between the four kinds of metaphors indicate
at least that
students differ in what they think is a socially desirable
answer.
Another limitation is that we assessed metaphors just in one
context at one
point of time. So we cannot draw conclusions about whether
metaphors are stable
across contexts or over time, as conceptions would be.
Nevertheless, metaphors
seem to be a promising research tool which should receive
further attention for
research on conceptions of learning, because it seems indeed
to matter whether
students see learning as a matter of training their brains or
tending their
gardens.
Keypoints
Students’ “metaphors
of learning” discriminated
between different profiles of motivation, epistemological
beliefs and use of
learning strategies.
Different categories
of metaphors could be linked to
both learning patterns and approaches to learning.
Students describing
learning in terms of personality
development shared similarities with deep approach learners
and meaning-directed
learners.
Students focussing
in their metaphors on only the regulation
aspects of learning shared similarities with undirected
learners.
Students describing
learning in terms of knowledge acquisition
shared similarities either with rehearsal-directed learners
or with learners
with a strategic approach.
References
Biggs, J. B. (1987). Student
Approaches to Learning and Studying. Research Monograph.
Australian Council
for Educational Research Ltd., Radford House, Frederick St.,
Hawthorn 3122,
Australia.
Ben-Peretz, M., Mendelson, N., &
Kron, F. W. (2003). How
teachers in different educational contexts view their roles. Teaching
and
Teacher Education, 19(2), 277–290.
doi:10.1016/S0742-051X(02)00100-2
Black, M. (1993). More about metaphor.
In
A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought (2nd. edition) (pp. 19–41).
Bullough, R. V. (1991). Exploring
personal teaching metaphors in preservice teacher education. Journal
of
Teacher Education, 42(1), 43–51.
doi: 10.1177/002248719104200107
Chi, M. T. H. (1997). Quantifying
qualitative analyses of verbal data: A practical guide. The journal of the learning sciences, 6(3), 271–315. doi: 10.1207/s15327809jls0603_1
Clandinin, D. J. (1985).
Personal practical knowledge: A study of teachers' classroom
images. Curriculum
inquiry, 15(4), 361-385. doi: 10.2307/1179683
Collins, J. B., & Pratt, D. D.
(2011). The teaching perspectives inventory at 10 years and
100,000
respondents: Reliability and validity of a teacher self-report
inventory. Adult Education Quarterly
November, 61(4), p.
358-375. 10.1177/0741713610392763.
Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (2003). Intrinsic motivation
inventory (IMI).
Retrieved in Oct.
2012 from
http://www.selfdeterminationtheory.org/questionnaires/
Entwistle, N. J., & Peterson, E. R.
(2004). Conceptions
of learning and knowledge in higher education: Relationships
with study
behaviour and influences of learning environments. International Journal of Educational Research, 41(6), 407–428. doi:10.1016/j.ijer.2005.08.009
Entwistle, N. J.,
& Ramsden, P. (1983). Understanding student
learning. London and Canberra: Croom
Helm.
Entwistle, N., & McCune, V. (2013).
The disposition to understand for oneself at university:
Integrating learning
processes with motivation and metacognition. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(2), 267–279. doi:10.1111/bjep.12010
Entwistle, N., Tait, H., & McCune,
V.
(2000). Patterns of response to an approaches to studying
inventory across
contrasting groups and contexts. European
Journal of Psychology of Education, 15(1),
33–48. doi:10.1007/BF03173165
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J.
(1980).
Analogical problem solving. Cognitive psychology, 12(3),
306–355.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90013-4
Gentner, D., & Holyoak, K. J.
(1997).
Reasoning and learning by analogy: Introduction. American
Psychologist, 52(1),
32–34. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.1.32
Gow, L., & Kember, D. (1993).
Conceptions of teaching and their relationship to student
learning. British
Journal of Educational Psychology,
63(1), 20-23. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1993.tb01039.x
Haser, V. (2005). Metaphor,
metonymy,
and experientialist philosophy: Challenging cognitive
semantics (Vol. 49).
Walter de Gruyter.
Inbar, D. E. (1996). The free
educational
prison: metaphors and images. Educational
Research, 38(1),
77–92.
doi: 10.1080/0013188960380106
Köller, O.,
Watermann, R., Trautwein, U. & Lüdtke, O. (2004). Wege
zur Hochschulreife
in Baden-Württemberg: TOSCA—Eine Untersuchung an allgemein
bildenden und
beruflichen Gymnasien. Opladen:
Leske + Budrich.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980).
Conceptual Metaphor in Everyday Language. The
Journal of Philosophy, 77(8),
453–486. doi: 10.2307/2025464
Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., &
Keefer, L. A. (2010). A
metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological bulletin,
136(6), 1045-1067.
10.1037/a0020970
Leavy, A. M., McSorley, F. A., &
Boté, L. A. (2007). An examination of what metaphor
construction reveals about
the evolution of preservice teachers’ beliefs about teaching
and learning. Teaching
and Teacher Education, 23(7), 1217–1233.
doi:
10.1016/j.tate.2006.07.016
Lehmann, B. (2012). Entwicklung eines
Instruments zur Erfassung unterrichtsbezogener Metaphern
[Development of an
instrument for assessing teaching-related metaphors]. In:
Faßhauer, U.,
Fürstenau, B., Wuttke, E. (Eds.): Berufs-
und wirtschaftspädagogische Analysen – aktuelle Forschungen
zur beruflichen
Bildung. [Analyses
in pedagogy of economics and
vocation – current research in vocational training]
(p.127-139) Opladen: Leske + Budrich.
Löfström, E., Nevgi, A., Wegner, E.,
& Karm, M. (2015). Images
In Research on Teaching and Learning in Higher Education. In:
J. Huisman &
M. Tight (Eds.), Theory and Method in Higher Education Research,
Volume 1. (p.
191-212). Emerald
Group Publishing Limited.
Löfström, E., & Poom-Valickis, K.
(2013). Beliefs about teaching: Persistent or malleable? A
longitudinal study
of prospective student teachers' beliefs. Teaching and
Teacher Education,
35, 104–113. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2013.06.004
Mahlios, M., Massengill‐Shaw, D., &
Barry, A.
(2010). Making sense of teaching through metaphors: A review
across three
studies. Teachers and
Teaching: theory and practice, 16(1), 49–71.
doi:10.1080/13540600903475645
Marsch, S. (2009). Metaphern des Lehrens und Lernens: vom Denken, Reden
und Handeln bei Biologielehrern.
[Metaphors of
Teaching and Learning:
Thinking, Talking and Practice of Biology Teachers.]
(Dissertation) Freie
Universität Berlin, Berlin.
Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (1976). On
Qualitative Differences in Learning: I—Outcome and process British Journal
of Educational Psychology,
46(1), 4–11. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1976.tb02980.x
Murphy, G. L. (1996). On metaphoric
representation. Cognition, 60(2), 173-204.
10.1016/0010-0277(96)00711-1
Pajares, F. M. (1992). Teachers'
beliefs
and educational research: Cleaning up a messy construct. Review of Educational Research, 62(3),
307. doi: 10.3102/00346543062003307
Parpala, A., Lindblom-Ylänne, S.,
Komulainen, E., Litmanen, T., & Hirsto, L. (2010).
Students' approaches to
learning and their experiences of the teaching–learning
environment in
different disciplines. British
Journal of
Educational Psychology, 80(2),
269–282. doi:10.1348/000709909X476946
Patchen, T., & Crawford, T. (2011).
From Gardeners to Tour Guides: The Epistemological Struggle
Revealed in
Teacher-Generated Metaphors of Teaching. Journal of
Teacher Education, 62(3),
286–298. doi:10.1177/0022487110396716
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F.,
Garcia,
T., & Mckeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and Predictive
Validity of the
Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (Mslq). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(3), 801–813. doi:10.1177/0013164493053003024
Richardson, J. T. E. (2007). Mental
models of learning in distance education. British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(2), 253–270.
doi:10.1348/000709906X110557
Richardson, J. T. E. (2011). Approaches
to studying, conceptions of learning and learning styles in
higher education. Learning
and Individual Differences, 21(3), 288–293. doi:
10.1016/j.lindif.2010.11.015
Saban, A., Kocbeker, B. N., &
Saban,
A. (2007). Prospective teachers' conceptions of teaching and
learning revealed
through metaphor analysis. Learning and
Instruction, 17(2),
123–139.
doi:10.1016/j.learninginstruc.2007.01.003
Säljö, R. (1979). Learning about
learning. Higher Education, 8(4), 443-451. 10.1007/BF01680533
Sfard, A. (1998). On two metaphors for
learning and the dangers of choosing just one. Educational
researcher, 27(2),
4-13. 10.3102/0013189X027002004
Thomas, G. P., & McRobbie, C. J.
(1999). Using metaphor to probe students' conceptions of
chemistry learning. International
journal of science education,
21(6), 667–685.
doi:10.1080/095006999290507
Vanthournout, G., Donche, V., Gijbels,
D., & van Petegem, P. (2014). (Dis)similarities in
research on learning
approaches and learning patterns. In D. Gijbels, V. Donche, J.
T. E.
Richardson, & J. D. Vermunt (Eds.), Learning
Patterns in Higher Education: Dimensions and research
perspectives (pp.11-32).
New York, Oxford: Routledge.
Vermunt, J.D. (1994). Scoring key for
the
inventory of learning styles (ILS) in higher education.
Tilburg: Tilburg
University.
Vermunt, J. D. (1996). Metacognitive,
Cognitive and Affective Aspects of Learning Styles and
Strategies: A
Phenomenographic Analysis. Higher
Education, 31(1),
25–50.
doi:10.2307/3447707
Vermunt, J. D,
& Vermetten, Y. (2004). Patterns
in Student Learning: Relationships Between Learning
Strategies, Conceptions of
Learning, and Learning Orientations. Educational
Psychology Review, 16(4),
359–384. doi:10.1007/s10648-004-0005-y
Vermetten, Y. J.,
Lodewijks, H. G., & Vermunt, J. D. (1999). Consistency and
variability of
learning strategies in different university courses. Higher
Education, 37(1),
1-21. doi:10.1023/A:1003573727713
Virtanen, V., & Lindblom-Ylänne, S.
(2010). University students’ and teachers’ conceptions of
teaching and learning
in the biosciences. Instructional
Science,
38(4), 355–370. doi:
10.1007/s11251-008-9088-z
Visser-Wijnveen, G., van Driel, J., van
der Rijst, R., Verloop, N., & Visser, A. (2009). The
relationship between
academics' conceptions of knowledge, research and teaching–a
metaphor study. Teaching
in Higher Education, 14(6), 673–686. doi:
10.1080/13562510903315340
Wegner, E., &
Nückles, M. (2015a). Knowledge
acquisition or participation in communities of practice?
Academics’ metaphors
of teaching and learning at the university. Studies
in Higher Education, 40(4),
624-643. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2013.842213
Wegner, E. & Nückles, M. (2015b).
From
eating to discovering: How metaphors of learning change during
students’
enculturation. Zeitschrift
für
Hochschulentwicklung, 10 (4),
145-166.
Wild, K.-P.,
& Schiefele, U. (1994). Lernstrategien im Studium:
Ergebnisse zur
Faktorenstruktur und Reliabilität eines neuen Fragebogens:
[Learning strategies
of university students: Factor structure and reliability of a
new
questionnaire.]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und
Diagnostische Psychologie,
15(4), 185–200.
Zapata, G. C., & Lacorte, M.
(2007). Preservice and
Inservice Instructors' Metaphorical Constructions of Second
Language Teachers. Foreign
Language Annals, 40(3), 521–534.
doi:10.1111/j.1944-9720.2007.tb02873.x