Visual and
Analytic Strategies in Geometry
George Kospentarisa,
Stella Vosniadoub, Smaragda Kazic,
Emilian Thanoud
aNational and
Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece
bThe Flinders
University of South Australia, Australia and National and
Kapodistrian
University of Athens, Greece
cPanteion
University of Social and Political Sciences, Greece
dNational and
Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece
Article received 13 November /
revised 28 December / accepted 19 January / available
online 3 March
Abstract
We argue that there
is an increasing reliance on analytic strategies compared to
visuo-spatial
strategies, which is related to geometry expertise and not
on individual
differences in cognitive style. A Visual/Analytic Strategy
Test (VAST) was
developed to investigate the use of visuo-spatial and
analytic strategies in
geometry in 30 mathematics teachers and 134 11th
grade students.
Students’ performance in the VAST was also compared to
performance in tests of
visuo-spatial abilities, of abstract reasoning, and of
geometrical knowledge.
The results showed high performance of all the participants
in the VAST items
that could be solved by relying on visuo-spatial strategies.
However, only the
math teachers showed high performance in the VAST items that
required the
application of analytic geometrical strategies. There were
high correlations
between the students’ performance in the tests of
visuo-spatial and abstract
reasoning abilities and the VAST Analytic Strategies scale,
but the
contribution of these tests to the VAST analytic performance
became
statistically insignificant when geometrical knowledge was
used as a mediating
factor. The implications of this work for the learning and
assessment of
geometrical knowledge are discussed.
Keywords:
Geometry
learning and
instruction; visual-spatial reasoning; analytic
strategies; assessment of geometry
Corresponding
author: George Kospentaris, aNational
and Kapodistrian University of Athens,
1.
Introduction
In
recent years research has accumulated showing that spatial
thinking is central
to success in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics,
the so-called
STEM disciplines. Spatial
thinking
is thinking about the location of objects and their relations
and requires both
visuo-spatial ability – the ability to mentally visualize the
rotation of
objects – and spatial abstract reasoning – the ability to
identify analogical
relations amongst patterns (see Newcombe, 2010; Wai, Lubinski,
& Benbow,
2009). There is convincing evidence that there are important
individual
differences in spatial thinking and that spatial thinking
abilities can predict
success in STEM disciplines (Hegarty & Waller, 2006; Wai, et
al., 2009).
Particularly impressive are the analyses of large data sets
showing that people
with high scores on tests of spatial thinking in high school are
more
interested in science and math, are more likely to get advanced
degrees in
STEM, and are more likely to pursue STEM careers (Shea,
Lubinski, & Benbow, 2001; Wai
et al., 2009). This has led to an increase in training studies
that
aim at improving spatial thinking as a means of improving
performance in STEM
disciplines (Sanchez,
2012; Uttal et al., 2013).
There
is little doubt that much of the problem solving done in
science, mathematics
and engineering requires the use of spatial thinking (Kozhevnikov,
Motes, & Hegarty, 2007; Stieff,
2007, Zazkis, Dubinsky, & Dautermann, 1996). In Euclidean
geometry, where
figures are the main objects of study, the role of spatial
thinking is of
utmost importance. Visualizing the shapes and their relation is
a standard
prerequisite for the understanding of geometrical propositions
(Battista,
2007). Although a great deal of this spatial thinking can be
achieved using
visuo-spatial strategies – i.e., strategies that allow
individuals to obtain
spatial information from immediate perceptual processes –
spatial thinking can
also be achieved using analytic strategies, where rules provide
access to
spatial information without recourse to visual perception and
mental animation
(Stieff, 2007). Zazkis et al. (1996) showed that the majority of
the university
students participating in abstract algebra courses used a
combination of
visuo-spatial and analytic strategies (see also Schwartz &
Black, 1996;
Stieff, 2007).
The
use of visuo-spatial vs. analytic strategies has been
predominately examined
from an individual differences point of view, as an individual
characteristic
or a cognitive style (Eisenberg & Dreyfus, 1991;
Pitta-Pantazi &
Christou, 2009). This emphasis on individual differences has
obscured the fact
that reliance on analytic strategies also characterizes the
acquisition of
expertise in many domains of STEM. As expertise is acquired,
problem solving
increasingly relies on specialized, domain-specific, rule-based,
analytic
approaches compared to visual, perceptual information and mental
rotation. For
example, in the domain of organic
chemistry expert chemists develop a predilection for analytic
strategies to
solve chemistry tasks (Stieff, 2007).
In
geometry, reliance on visuo-spatial strategies seems to coincide
with the Level
1 of the van Hiele (1986) theory in geometrical thinking. At
later levels
geometrical thinking increasingly requires an understanding of
the logical
systems that geometry represents. In geometry, shapes are
represented by a set
of properties and their relations and geometrical thinking is
characterized by
the formal manipulation of a logical system. Thus, when
geometrical expertise
is achieved, geometrical thinking relies increasingly on
analytical formal
processes based on geometrical knowledge. The purpose of the
present research is
to develop a task that can differentiate visual from analytic
reasoning in
geometry -- a Visual/Analytic Strategy Task (VAST) -- and to
validate it by
comparing novices to experts in geometry. In the next section we
present a
summary review of the literature on geometrical thinking and
define and explain
our theoretical position with respect to the use of
visuo-spatial and analytic
strategies.
1.1
Geometrical
thinking
Piaget
and his collaborators (Piaget, Inhelder, & Szeminska,
1948/1960; Piaget
& Inhelder, 1948/1967) were the first to study the
psychological
foundations of geometrical thinking and to propose that it
develops in four
sequential and hierarchical stages[1].
In subsequent years, van Hiele (1986) argued that there are
five, qualitatively
distinct, hierarchical levels of thought in geometry. In
contrast to Piaget, van Hiele strongly emphasized
the crucial role of school instruction in the acquisition of
geometrical
knowledge (van Hiele, 1986, p. 65-66). More recently, Houdement
and
Kuzniak (2003) proposed (on theoretical grounds) that the five
van Hiele levels
can be reduced to three Kuhnian-like paradigms: Geometry
1-Natural Geometry;
Geometry II- Natural Axiomatic Geometry, and Geometry
III – Formalist
Axiomatic Geometry.
Empirical
research so far has failed to confirm the predictions of the van
Hiele theory
that students move through discrete levels of geometrical
thought, each
characterized by different internal conceptual organization
(Battista, 2007).
It appears instead that students oscillate between different
levels of
geometric understanding depending on the context and the nature
of the problems
to be solved. For this reason some researchers have argued that
although there
might be different levels of geometric thinking as identified by
van Hiele,
these do not represent distinct stages but develop in parallel
and without
discontinuities between them (Clements & Battista, 2001;
Lehrer, Jenkins, & Osana, 1998).
It
follows from the above that we need a theoretical framework that
can account for
the considerable conceptual re-organizations that take place in
the process of
acquiring and using geometrical knowledge without posing the
existence of
hierarchical and well-defined distinct stages. For these reason,
it is proposed
here that it might be fruitful to examine geometrical thinking
from a
conceptual change point of view, and that the framework theory
(FT) approach to
conceptual change (Vosniadou, 2013; Vosniadou & Skopeliti,
2014) can serve
as an anchor for examining changes in geometrical knowledge
after exposure to
instruction. The FT belongs to a class of conceptual change
approaches known as
‘theory-theory’ (Carey, 2009), but also differs from them in
important ways.
Briefly, the FT claims that (a) there are systems of core
cognition that bootstrap
cognitive development (Carey, 2009; Spelke & Kinzler, 2007),
without making
strong nativist interpretations of early infant competencies[2],
and (b) that conceptual development consists of episodes of
qualitative change,
which, however, are not discontinuous or stage like. Rather,
conceptual change
is seen as a slow and gradual learning process greatly
facilitated by
sociocultural and educational inputs. According to the FT, the
same
constructive-type mechanisms that are involved in all learning
processes are
also involved in conceptual change processes often producing
fragmentation and
misconceptions, but eventually having the potential to lead to
qualitatively
different conceptual organizations (Vosniadou & Skopeliti,
2014). Finally,
the FT claims that initial systems of thought continue to exist
and influence
thinking, even after instruction-induced conceptual changes have
occurred
(Shtulman & Varcarcel, 2012; Vosniadou et al., 2015).
Seen
from this theoretical perspective, it is argued that geometrical
knowledge is
originally built on two core cognitive systems (spatial and
numerical) that
rely on visuo-spatial information (Newcombe & Frick, 2010;
Spelke, Lee,
& Izard, 2010), but that it gradually develops through
systematic
instruction to rely on more analytic strategies based on formal
geometrical
knowledge. In other words, we claim that there is a growing
reliance on
analytic strategies in geometric thinking with the acquisition
of expertise,
and that the systematic use of analytic strategies is a product
of conceptual
changes that take place in the subject-matter area of geometry.
We do not claim
that visuo-spatial strategies become extinct and that experts
rely on analytic
strategies only. Unlike
stage theories,
we argue that the initial, visuo-spatial, approach to geometry
is not
supplanted by the analytic one, but continues to exist and to be
used when
contextually appropriate. The ability to systematically employ
analytic
strategies in geometry, however, is a major intellectual
achievement and not a
matter of individual differences in cognitive style. It is the
product of a
conceptual change which takes place over many years and which
requires the
acquisition of new concepts and new forms of geometrical
thinking.
We
believe that many geometry education researchers would agree
with this account
of the development of geometrical knowledge. Geometry is
undeniably a formal
system and geometric reasoning consists of using this formal
system to reason
about shape and space. According to Battista (2007), underlying
this formal
system is a ‘primitive’ system of visuo-spatial thinking
allowing individuals
to ‘see’, inspect, and reflect on spatial objects, images,
relationships and
transformations (p. 843). This ‘primitive’ system is
characterized by what he
calls ‘perceptual objects’ – i.e., mental entities perceived by
an individual
when viewing physical objects in the real world, including
geometrical
diagrams. In contrast, expert geometrical knowledge operates on
‘conceptual
objects’ – i.e., abstract, completely idealized and general
mental entities
based on ‘formal’ categories, which are explicitly circumscribed
according to
verbally stated, property-based definitions.
The difference between a geometric diagram and a figure
captures this
basic dichotomy: the former is a material entity, a concrete
case that
imperfectly represents the abstract concept, while the latter is
a theoretical,
ideal object without any physical properties. Similarly,
Fischbein (1993)
argues that experts in geometry form and reason with ‘figural
concepts’. A
figural concept is controlled by logical rules in the context of
an axiomatic
system but is also a mental entity, an image with a
spatial-figural content,
although devoid of any concrete sensorial properties (Fischbein,
1993, p. 148).
Battista’s
(2007) and Fischbein’s (1993)
arguments are consistent with the proposal that there are
ontological
and representational shifts that take place in the development
of geometrical
knowledge analogous in some respects to the ontological shifts
that take place
in learning science (Chi, 2008; Vosniadou, 2013). For example, a
circle, this
quite familiar shape, changes from a visual gestalt (Figure 1a)
and becomes the
locus of all plane points characterized with the property that
they are equidistant
from its center (Figure 1b), or, in the conceptual frame of
analytic geometry,
to an equation (the plane points satisfying x2+y2
=r2,
Figure 1c). In addition, the theoretical explanations in the
domain also
change. At the beginning, geometrical propositions are mainly
inductive
generalizations based on empirical observations and
experimentation with
perceptual objects and not on proofs and deductive procedures
based on accepted
axioms and previously proven propositions.
Figure 1. Changes
in the representation of
the circle. (see pdf)
It
could be argued that the above arguments would also be
acceptable by stage
theories, such as the van Hiele theory. If this is the case,
then what can the
FT offer in our theoretical understanding of geometrical
expertise? Although some
stage theories allow for intra-individual differences across
tasks (a
phenomenon known as decalage –Piaget & Inhelder, 1948/1967),
they
nevertheless assume that a) the new forms of thinking that
develop in geometry
gradually transform and eventually replace the ‘primitive’
visuo-spatial system
with a more advanced system of thought based on analytical,
formal knowledge,
and b) that this process leads to distinct, qualitatively
different stages in
students’ thinking. From the perspective of the FT, however,
knowledge
acquisition does not proceed through hierarchical and well
defined distinct
stages, but through the gradual assimilation of the new
information into the
initial, ‘primitive’ system, creating in the process inert
knowledge,
fragmentation, and misconceptions, many of which are ‘synthetic’
conceptions[3]
(Vosniadou & Skopeliti, 2014). Although there is an order of
acquisition in
this conceptual development and some learning progressions can
be identified,
these cannot be characterized as ‘stages’, both because they
cannot be clearly
identified as such, and because the ‘primitive’ -
visuo-spatially-based system
– is not eradicated but continues to co-exist with the formal,
analytical modes
of thought (Shtulman & Valcarcel, 2012).
The
purpose of the present research is not to test a full-blown
theory of
geometrical thinking, but to start in this direction by
developing a valid
task that can help us distinguish the use of visuo-spatial from
analytic
strategies in geometry. In
the next
section the rationale behind the development of the
Visual/Analytic Strategy
Task is described.
1.2
The
Visual/Analytic Strategy Task (VAST).
Several
tasks have been developed over the years to test students’
movement from the
visual to the descriptive/analytic van Hiele level (e.g., Burger
& Shaughnessy,
1986; Gutiérrez & Jaime, 1998; Lynn & Lynch, 2010;
Usiskin, 1982). The main limitation of such
tasks is that they either favour the recall or recognition of
definitions of
shapes and their properties over their understanding and their
application in
novel situations, or that they do not require thinking based on
more
sophisticated, relational properties (Battista (2007).
In
addition to the above, there are several other standardized
geometry tests that
assess students’ level of geometrical knowledge, such as the
California
Standards Geometry Test (CSGT, 2009). These standardized tests
examine mainly
the extent to which students can perform school procedures,
e.g., to apply a
known formula for some computation within a narrow formal
context, which often
imposes a particular solution method. Thus it remains unclear
whether the
students who succeed in these tests would present the same level
of geometry
knowledge in situations where the test format would not be
similar to the way
they have been taught.
In the
present research a different method to measure visual and
analytic reasoning
was developed, based on the following considerations: First, we
avoided setting our task in
the typical geometry textbook style that could suggest deductive
requirements
and delimit visualization or measuring. Second, we did not
impose a particular
solution method to the solver but rather selected problems that
could be solved
using either visuo-spatial or analytic strategies, so that we
could
investigate spontaneous strategy choice. Third, we wanted to
investigate not only
whether students are able to use analytic strategies but also
whether they are
able to do so in situations that require them to inhibit
visual-perceptual
information processing and reason instead along formal geometric
lines. Thus, a
task was needed in which the perceptual difficulty of comparing
shapes would be
intensive and where the use of analytic strategies would lead to
conclusions sometimes conflicting with visual-perceptual
information.
The
above theoretical considerations led to the development of the
present
Visual/Analytic Strategy Task (VAST). The VAST is a
verbal/picture verification
task. The
participants are presented with a geometrical configuration that
includes two
shapes and are asked to decide whether a verbal statement that
states that these
shapes are congruent (Congruence domain), similar (Similarity
domain), or
occupy the same area (Area domain), is true or false. As shown
in Figure 2,
there are four types of configuration conditions in each
geometrical domain:
(a) the ‘Appearance+/Reality+’ condition where the two shapes
both appear to be
and are indeed congruent, similar or area equivalent; (b) The
‘Appearance-/Reality-’ condition where the two shapes neither
appear nor are
congruent, similar or area equivalent; (c) The
‘Appearance+/Reality-’ condition
where the two shapes appear to be but are not congruent, similar
or area
equivalent; and (d) The ‘Appearance-/Reality+’ condition where the two shapes
do not appear to be but
are congruent, similar or area equivalent. On the top of each
configuration
there is a verbal statement, such as, for instance, ‘The lengths
of the routes
are the same’ (See Figure 2, upper row). The participants are
asked to decide whether
this statement is true or false with respect to the geometrical
configuration
to which it refers.
In all
three geometrical domains, the conditions (a) and (b) involve
items purposely
designed to be solved by visual estimation alone and which are
consistent with
the adoption of either a visual or an analytic geometric
strategy (thereafter
the VAST Consistent Subscale, or VAST-ConS). The conditions (c)
and (d) are
inconsistent with reliance on visual estimation alone and
require for their
correct solution reliance on geometrical knowledge and the
adoption of analytic
strategies (thereafter the VAST-InconS). The geometrical
knowledge required
involves either measurement and empirical confirmation or
Euclidean deductive
argumentation.
More specifically, in the
case
of the geometrical configurations a1 and a2 in Figure 2, the
conclusions that
the two routes are equal (in a1) and unequal (in a2) can be
reliably achieved
through visual-spatial inspection. They can also be deduced on
the basis of
known geometrical properties: In a1, the conclusion of equality
can be deduced
from the congruence of the corresponding line segments, which
are the opposite
sides of the formed rectangles. In a2, the conclusion that
Nick’s path is
shorter than John’s path can be deduced from the geometrical
axiom of triangle
inequality – that the hypotenuse is always shorter than the sum
of the right
angled segments. In the case of the geometrical configuration a3
and a4,
however, the conclusions cannot be deduced by using visual
strategies, but only
through reliance on geometrical knowledge. In a3, in order to
deduce the
inequality of length line segments, one has to compute the
hypotenuses of the
formed right triangles and compare the oblique line segments
with the vertical
or horizontal ones. In a4 by drawing horizontal and vertical
lines, the
segments forming the zigzag “John’s route” are equal to the
corresponding
segments forming the direct “Nick’s route”, as opposite sides of
rectangles.
The above rationale applies to all items of the test.
Figure 2.
Sample items from the Visual/Analytic Shift
Test (VAST). (see pdf)
1.3
Questions
and Hypotheses of the Present Study
Our purpose in the present
study was to examine if the VAST is a reliable and valid test of
visual and
analytic reasoning in geometry. With respect to reliability, we
wanted to find
out whether Kuder-Richardson (K-R) reliability index was
acceptable across the different
sub-scales (Hypothesis 1).
With respect to validity,
and
in view of our theoretical position that the use of visual and
analytic
strategies is related to geometry expertise, we wanted to find
out if the VAST
would be able to differentiate the performance of experts in
geometry from that
of novices. For
this reason, the VAST
was administered to a group of mathematics teachers with
extensive experience
in teaching geometry and to a group of 11th grade
students who had
been exposed to Euclidean geometry teaching. We hypothesized
that if the VAST
is a good test of the use of visual and analytic strategies,
then the
mathematics teachers should have high scores both in the
Vast-ConS and in the
VAST-InconS because of their expertise in geometry. On the
contrary, the
students would obtain high scores only in the VAST-ConS, which
can be solved
with visuo-spatial strategies and not in the VAST-InconS, which
requires
reliance on analytic strategies based on geometrical knowledge
and the
inhibition of visual estimation (Hypothesis 2).
The
above hypotheses are different from what would be expected
assuming that performance
in the VAST is related only to individual differences in
strategy use as
opposed to geometry expertise. Individual differences in
strategy use would not
predict systematic differences in the performance of the high
school students.
Rather, some participants should do better in the visuo-spatial
items and some
others in the analytic items, regardless of their geometry
knowledge
(Hypothesis 3).
Hypothesis
4 concerned the relation between the VAST-InconS and geometrical
knowledge, as
measured by school grades in geometry (GG) and performance in a
standardized
test of geometrical knowledge (the California Standards Geometry
Test -
CSGT). High
correlations were predicted
between performance in the VAST-InconS, CSGT and GG, because
they are all
alternative measures of geometrical problem solving and
geometrical knowledge.
Finally,
we investigated the relation between VAST-IconS and two
cognitive abilities
that comprise spatial thinking: (i) abstract
reasoning ability, i.e., the ability to identify
patterns, analogical
relationships and logical rules, and (ii) visuo-spatial
ability – i.e., the ability to mentally visualize the
rotation of objects.
In view of the well-documented findings in the literature that
spatial thinking
is strongly related with students’ performance in STEM subjects,
we
hypothesized that performance in the VAST-InconS should
correlate positively
with performance in the these two tests of spatial thinking
(Hypothesis 5).
However, in accordance with our theoretical position, namely
that it is the
acquisition of geometrical knowledge that leads to the use of
analytic
strategies, we expected that geometry knowledge (as measured by
the CSGT) would
significantly contribute to VAST-InconS performance, reducing
the influence of
the spatial thinking factor (Hypothesis 6).
2.
Method
2.1
Participants
The
participants included 30 mathematics teachers (age
range 30-55 years, 18 men) and 134
11th grade students (age range 16.4-17.5 years, 71
boys). The
mathematics teachers had considerable experience teaching high
school geometry.
The students were of middle-class backgrounds, came from two
different schools,
had four different geometry teachers, and were towards the end
of a five-year
course in geometry[4].
2.2
Materials
The Visual Analytic Shift Test (VAST)
consisted of four items for
each geometrical domain counterbalanced across the four
conditions described
earlier. Thus, there were a total of 48 items (4 items for each
geometrical
domain × 3 domains × 4 conditions), randomly ordered.
The
California Standards Geometry Test (CSGT) consisted of
15 items (5 for
each geometrical domain) selected from the overall 96 items of
the California
Standards Geometry Test sample released in 2009 (http://www.cde.ca.gov/Ta/tg/sr/documents/cstrtqgeomapr15.pdf).
The selection was made on
the basis of the relevance of each item to the national geometry
curriculum.
The Purdue
Visualization of Rotations Test (ROT) is a test that
determines how well one can visualize
the rotation of three- dimensional objects. It is among the
tests of spatial
thinking less likely to be contaminated by analytical abilities.
To restrict analytical
processing, a time limit of 10 minutes for the 20-item version
of this test was
strictly enforced (Bodner & Guay, 1997).
The Abstract Reasoning
Test
(ART) is one
of the tests of spatial thinking used by Wai et al., (2009). It
is a non-verbal
measure of fluid intelligence, consisting of 15 items.
School grades in Geometry were collected for all students participating in
the study.
2.3
Procedure
The
VAST was administered to the mathematics teachers individually
in their school
office. The VAST and the CSGT were administered to the students
as a group test
during a 45-minute class session. Their order of presentation
was
counterbalanced. The students were instructed to answer the VAST
and CSGT items
using whatever method they found suitable.
Formulas were provided to students individually, if they
asked for them.
The ROT and ART were administered to small groups of students in
the school
computer lab. Completion of the electronic tests required
approximately 30
minutes.
3.
Results
3.1
Reliability
indices
Since
all measures were binominal, the
Kuder-Richardson (K-R) reliability test was applied. The results
showed
that the reliability of the two subscales was acceptable
(VAST-ConS, Kuder-Richardson
(K-R) = .70;
VAST-InconS, Kuder-Richardson (K-R) = .75). Reliability of the
rest scales
are as follows: CSGT (Kuder-Richardson (K-R)= .76, range of mean
percentage performance: 13.33-100.00, Mean= 63.82, StD= 22.35),
ROT (Kuder-Richardson
(K-R)= .74,
range: 1-20, Mean= 8.59, StD= 3.81), and ART (Kuder-Richardson
(K-R)= .57,
range: 5-15, Mean= 9.89, StD= 2.49).
3.2
Performance
of the experts vs novices
In order to examine the
effect
of the visual vs. analytic component on the participants’
performance,
two composite scores were computed: The mean percentage
performances in the VAST Consistent
Subscale (VAST-ConS) and in the VAST Inconsitent Subscale
(VAST-InconS).
Examination of the mean and standard deviation of the
performance on the
VAST-ConS, showed that the scale almost reached a ceiling effect
(Mean
Percentage= 83.86, see Table 1). Thus, as was planned, this
subscale consisted
of easy items that could be successfully solved by the math
teachers as well as
by the students. Given that normality assumptions did not hold
for this
particular subscale, no parametric tests were applied.
Table 1
Means
and StD as a function of Expertise and Item type (VAST-ConS
vs. VAST-InconS) of
the VAST
Participants |
Item
Type |
|
|
|||
|
VAST-ConS |
VAST-InconS |
Total |
|||
|
Mean |
StD |
Mean |
StD |
Mean |
StD |
Teachers |
92.000 |
7.575 |
78.841 |
11.685 |
85.392 |
7.525 |
Students |
81.637 |
12.454 |
50.751 |
15.314 |
66.175 |
10.312 |
Total |
83.857 |
12.321 |
56.770 |
18.606 |
70.293 |
12.566 |
A t-test for independent
samples was applied on VAST-InconS performance. Results showed
significant
difference between the two groups [t(138)= -9.324, p<.001,
Mean= 50.75, for
the students, and Mean= 78.84, for the math teachers]. The math
teachers
answered correctly almost all of the items in the VAST-InconS,
whereas the 11th
graders had considerable difficulty with the VAST-InconS. In
agreement with
Hypothesis 2, teachers’ and students’ performance was clearly
differentiated in
the VAST-InconS, where the performance of students was
considerably lower than
that of the teachers.
In
order to examine Hypothesis 3, we plotted the students’
individual mean
percentage performance in the VAST-ConS and the VAST-InconS.
Figure 3 shows the
mean percent score of each participant on the y-axis. As can be
seen, it was not the
case that some participants
performed well in the VAST-ConS and others in the VAST-InconS,
as would have
been predicted by the individual differences/cognitive style
hypothesis. With
very few exceptions, the items in the VAST-ConS that could have
been solved
using visuo-spatial strategies were much easier for each
individual participant
than the items in the VAST-InconS, which required recourse to
analytic
strategies. It can also be seen, that many students performed
well only in the
case of the VAST-ConS.
Figure 3. Individuals’
mean
performance in the VAST-ConS and the VAST-InconS. The
performance of the
teachers is shown above the horizontal line. (see pdf)
3.3
Relations
between the VAST-ConS, VAST-InconS CSGT, GG ROT and ART
Due to the violation of the
normality assumption of the VastConS, a Spearman’s rho
correlation analysis was
performed on the mean scores of the VastConS with the
VastInconS, CSGT, GG, ROT
and ART. Results showed that VastConS correlated moderately
only with ROT
(rho=.190, p=.047), whereas all other correlations were
insignificant (with
VastInconS, rho=.136, with CSGT, rho=.165, with GG, rho=-.054,
and with ART,
rho=.176).
3.4
Relations
between the VAST-InconS, CSGT and GG
In
order to examine Hypothesis 5, a correlation analysis (Pearson’s
r) was performed on the
mean percentage scores on the VAST-ConS, the VAST-InconS, ROT
(Cronbach alpha=
.74, range: 1-20, Mean= 8.59, StD= 3.81), and ART (Cronbach
alpha= .57, range:
5-15, Mean= 9.89, StD= 2.49). As predicted, the results showed
statistically
significant correlations between performance in the two VAST
subscales, ROT and
ART (Table 2).
3.5
Relations
between the VAST-InconS, ROT and ART
In
order to examine Hypothesis 5, a correlation analysis (Pearson’s
r) was performed on the
mean percentage scores on the
VAST-InconS, ROT (Cronbach alpha= .74, range: 1-20, Mean=
8.59, StD=
3.81), and ART (Cronbach alpha= .57, range: 5-15, Mean= 9.89,
StD= 2.49). As
predicted, the results showed statistically significant
correlations between
performance in the two VAST subscales, ROT and ART (Table 2).
Table 2
Correlation
between the measures of the study
|
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
1. VAST-InconS |
- |
|
|
|
|
2. CSGT |
.429** |
- |
|
|
|
3. GG |
.318** |
.575** |
- |
|
|
4. ROT |
.313** |
.357** |
.249** |
- |
|
5. ART |
.366** |
.414** |
.285** |
.327** |
- |
**.
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
*. Correlation
is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
A stepwise regression analysis was
applied with the purpose of examining in greater detail the
contributions of
the above-mentioned measures of this study to VAST-InconS
performance. The
measures were inserted in the analysis in the following order:
ROT, ART, and
CSGT. The order of insertion followed the theoretical rationale
of the present
study, that is, general visuo-spatial ability (ROT) was inserted
first, followed
by general abstract reasoning ability (ART), and, finally, by
performance on
CSGT, which incorporated all the above and included geometrical
knowledge. Based on
our theoretical analysis,
performance in CSGT should predict performance in the
VAST-InconS best.
Table 3
Results of
step-wise regression of ROT, ART, and CSGT on VAST-InconS
Model |
|
B |
Std. Error |
β |
t |
Sig. |
1 |
ROT |
1.258 |
.432 |
.295 |
2.909 |
.005 |
2 |
ROT |
.901 |
.430 |
.211 |
2.096 |
.039 |
|
ART |
1.772 |
.581 |
.307 |
3.048 |
.003 |
3 |
ROT |
.522 |
.442 |
.122 |
1.179 |
.242 |
|
ART |
1.223 |
.603 |
.212 |
2.027 |
.046 |
|
CSGT |
.196 |
.076 |
.282 |
2.561 |
.012 |
The results (see Table 3) showed that
all
three consecutive models had a good fit. For the first step
(ROT) [F (1, 89) =8.461, p= .005], for the second step (ROT and
ART) [F (2, 88) =
9.269, p< .001], and for the third step (ROT, ART and CSGT)
[F
(3, 88) = 8.755,
p< .001]. As it can be
seen in Table 3, when CSGT was entered in the model the
contribution of ROT
became non-significant (p= .242)
and the contribution of ART became marginally significant (p=
.046). These results fully confirmed
Hypothesis 6, indicating that geometrical knowledge, and not
general visuo-spatial
abilities and abstract spatial reasoning, accounted for the
visual/analytic
strategy shift as measured by the VAST-InconS.
In order to further validate the above
result, a mediation analysis of the patterns of relations was
applied on the
data (see Figure 4), by using AMOS (version SPSS21) through
bootstrapping
(number of bootstrap samples=2000, Bias corrected confidence
intervals= .95).
First, the direct relations between ROT and ART on the
VAST-InconS were
computed. For ROT and VAST: two-tailed significance p< .045,
and for ART and
VAST: two-tailed significance p< .019. Thus, the results
indicated that both
paths were significant. We then tested two models, one with the
CSGT and the
other with geometry grades as mediating variables.
When the CSGT was added as
a
mediating variable, the indirect effect (i.e., the mediating
path from ROT
through CSGT to VAST-InconS) was significant (p= .006), and so
was the
mediating path from ART through CSGT to VAST-InconS (p= .001).
Inspection of
the direct effects showed that both relations were completely
mediated by CSGT
(for the path between ROT and VAST-InconS, p= .143 and for the
path ART to
VAST-InconS, p= .133). The best model that resulted (see Figure
4), eliminating
only the direct relation from ROT to the Vast-InconS, had an
acceptable fit (χ2
(1)= 2.384, p= .123, CFI= . 977, Standardized RMR= .04).
Figure 4. Regression
weights of the
Mediation Analysis between ROT, ART, CSGT, and VAST-InconS.
When
geometry grades were treated as the mediating variable, the
indirect effect from ROT through geometry
grades to VAST-InconS was significant (p= .042), and so was the
mediating path
from ART through school grades to VAST-InconS
(p= .025). Inspection of the direct effects showed that
the relation
between ROT and the VAST-IconS was completely mediated by
geometry grades (for
the path between ROT and VAST-InconS p= .086), whereas the
relation between ART
and the VAST-InconS was partially mediated by grades (p= .048).
The final
model, after eliminating the direct relation between ROT and the
VAST-InconS
did not, however, show a good fit [χ2 (1)= 3.431, p=
.06, CFI= .942,
Standardized RMR= .05], since the value of χ2/df
exceeded the value of 3, and model’s p value was statistically
significant.
To
conclude and summarize, the results of the mediation analysis
confirmed the
hypothesis that performance on the VAST-InconS will be mediated
by geometrical
knowledge, particularly when geometrical knowledge was measured
by CSGT. In
addition, the model with the best fit still retained the direct
relation
between analytic reasoning as measured by ART and the
VAST-InconS.
4.
Discussion
Our
main purpose in this study was to develop and validate a task
that could
distinguish visuo-spatial from analytic reasoning in geometry.
As mentioned in
the introduction, there have been several attempts so far to
develop tasks that
capture the change from the visual to the descriptive/analytic
level in
geometry. These
previous attempts were
not very successful because they were based on the recall or
recognition of
definitions of shapes and their properties, did not require
thinking based on
relational properties, and/or did not require the application of
formal
geometrical thinking in novel situations.
The
VAST differs substantially from these previous attempts because
it avoids the
typical geometry-style problems that impose an analytical
solution method and
because it consists of tasks that can be solved using either
visuo-spatial or
analytic strategies. The greatest advantage of VAST, compared to
previous
tests, is that it focuses specifically on the antagonism between
the two
substantially different types of strategy in geometry and allows
us to check
individuals’ abilities to spontaneously choose and adequately
apply the
correct strategy. Finally, the VAST investigates the ability to
use analytic
strategies in situations where the visual element plays quite a
central role
and where visual information processing must be inhibited in
favour of formal
geometrical thinking.
The
results of the present study showed that the VAST consists of
items that have
good internal consistency.
Most
importantly the results show that the VAST is a valid test
because it can
differentiate geometry teachers from students and because it
correlates highly
with other tests of spatial thinking and geometrical knowledge.
4.1
Are
the differences in VAST performance related to geometry
expertise?
Despite
small differences in the ease or difficulty of the solution of
individual
items, the main pattern of results was the same: The items in
the VAST-ConS
that could be solved correctly using visuo-spatial strategies
were much easier
for all participants than the items in the VAST-InconS which
required the use
of analytic strategies and the inhibition of the visual element.
As
predicted (Hypothesis 2), the math teachers were able to solve
both the
VAST-ConS and the VAST-InconS. This result suggests that the
math teachers have
access to both visuo-spatial and analytic strategies. On the
contrary, the 11th grade students could easily solve only the
VAST-ConS, but
had great difficulty with the VAST-InconS. This finding
indicates that the
students relied predominantly on visuo-spatial strategies and
had difficulty in
employing analytic strategies when required by the task, further
confirming
Hypothesis 2.
The
finding that there were systematic differences in the
performance of the math
teachers and the students -- the math teachers performed well in
both the
VAST-ConS and the VAST-InconS, but the 11th grade students were
able to perform
well only in the VAST-ConS -- supports the argument that the use
of analytic
strategies is related to the acquisition of geometry expertise.
As explained in
the introduction, if the differences in VAST performance were
due to individual
differences in strategy use, then we should expect some
mathematics teachers
and some students to perform well in the VAST-ConS and others in
the
VAST-InconS (Hypothesis 3).
This was not
however the case. The items in the VAST-InconS were
systematically more
difficult than the items in the VAST-ConS both for teachers and
for students.
Moreover, the students performed well only in the VAST-ConS,
indicating that
the majority of the students were able to successfully apply
only simple
visuo-spatial strategies.
Additional
evidence in favour of the interpretation that the use of
analytic strategies
requiring geometrical knowledge in the VAST-Inconsistent
sub-scale comes from
the results of the mediation analysis which showed that
relations between
performance in the ROT and the VAST- Inconsistent subscale was
completely
mediated by performance in the CSGT. This means that high
performance in the
VAST-Inconsistent scale requires not just domain-general
analytic skills, but
domain-specific geometrical knowledge and ability to use
analytic thinking in a
geometrical context.
The
results showed considerable individual differences in the
performance of the
students in the VAST-InconS. These differences seem to be
related to
differences in geometry expertise within the student group. This
conclusion can
be deduced from the high correlations that were obtained between
performance in
the VAST-InconS, performance in the CSGT, and GG (Hypothesis 4). Students’ performance
in the VAST-ConS and
assumed ability to apply simple visuo-spatial strategies did not
correlate
significantly with their GG or with their performance in the
CSGT, confirming
Hypothesis 4, namely, that this performance does not necessarily
require
geometrical knowledge.
4.2
What
is the contribution of the cognitive skills involved in
spatial thinking in
VAST performance?
The
results of the present study showed high correlations between
performance in
the VAST-ConS and the VAST-InconS with both ROT and ART,
confirming the
prediction that spatial thinking as measured by tests of
visuo-spatial and
abstract (spatial) reasoning abilities contributes to students’
performance in
both scales of the VAST. However, performance in the VAST-InconS
was also
highly correlated with performance in the test of geometrical
knowledge (the
CSGT) and geometry grades. Most importantly the results of the
stepwise
regression confirmed that when the CSGT performance was inserted
in the last
step of analysis, the contribution of ROT and ART (which were
statistically
significant in the previous steps of the analysis), became non-
or marginally
significant. Furthermore, when a mediation analysis was applied
on the data,
the previously significant direct relations between performance
in the
VAST-InconS and ROT were mediated by the students’ geometrical
knowledge, as
measured by performance in the CSGT. Since the direct relations
between ART and
VAST-InconS were not eliminated, however, it might be the case
that analytic
abilities are directly contributing to performance in the
VAST-InconS.
To sum
up, the results indicate that the correct use of analytic
strategies cannot be
explained only on the basis of visuo-spatial abilities and
abstract reasoning,
i.e., the cognitive skills that comprise spatial thinking, but
requires the
accumulation of considerable geometrical knowledge.
4.3
Implications
for a theory of geometrical thinking
One of
the main reasons we developed the VAST was in order to show that
the learning
of geometry requires significant conceptual changes to take
place, and that
instruction-induced conceptual changes culminate in the ability
to use formal,
geometrical knowledge in problem-solving and in the flexible use
of
visual/spatial and analytic strategies appropriate in the given
contexts. The
role of instruction here is quite crucial. As Fischbein (1993)
stressed, “the
development of figural concepts generally is not a natural
process” (p. 161).
The present findings support the hypothesis that the use of
analytic strategies
in geometry is not a matter of individual differences in
cognitive style but a
major intellectual achievement, a conceptual change, which
requires the
acquisition of new forms of geometrical thinking.
The
application of analytic, formal geometrical knowledge in problem
solving does
not mean that visuo-spatial geometrical reasoning disappears.
The fact that the
math teachers could easily solve the items in the VAST-ConS
suggests that they
still had access to visual strategies. This issue needs to be
investigated
further, however, in view of the fact that analytic strategies
could be used in
the VAST-ConS also. Finally, a great deal more research is also
required to
investigate the hypotheses of the FT according to which the
processes of
acquisition of geometrical knowledge are slow and gradual rather
than sudden or
stage-like, and that these processes can give rise to
fragmentation and
synthetic models.
4.4
Implications
for learning and instruction
The
low performance of the students in the VAST-InconS after almost
five years
of instruction in geometry supports the argument that the
systematic use of
analytic strategies is a major intellectual achievement that
requires
considerable conceptual changes. It should be added here that
the 11th grade
students were in their 5th year of geometry
instruction, they had
completed a two year course in plane Eucleadian Geometry, which
was taught in a
formal manner and accompanied by many geometry problems to be
solved, and that
they had an additional year’s course in Analytic Geometry
designed for students
opting for university study in STEM subjects. Consequently, all
the students
had been taught the formulae and theorems required to answer
correctly all the
VAST items, both in the consistent and inconsistent sub-scales.
Thus,
we can conclude from the above that it is possible to have
acquired a great
deal of school-type knowledge in geometry and not yet function
at an analytic
level in geometric thinking. Although geometry instruction
focuses almost
exclusively on the acquisition of formal geometrical knowledge
and the application
of analytic strategies, it does not seem to be very successful
in transferring
to situations different from the narrow school context in which
it is taught
and in producing the necessary conceptual changes. This situation is
similar to what is happening
in other STEM domains, such as physics or chemistry, where
school instruction
often fails to produce the necessary conceptual changes (e.g.,
Clement, 1982;
diSessa, 1982). We hope that the present findings will further
sensitize
educators of the need to develop instruction that emphasizes not
the recall and
rigid application of formal definitions and rules but the
constructive, dynamic
activities of students that can help them understand how formal
definitions fit
with their visual-spatial experiences and representations of
geometrical shapes
(Fischbein, 1993; Kilpatrick, Hoyles, Skovsmose, & Valero,
2005;
Lehrer Jenkins & Osana, 1998).
4.5
Limitations
of the present study and future research
The
present work has a number of limitations and leaves several open
questions to
be answered by future research.
First,
the sample of the present study is small when attempting to
validate a new
measure such as the VAST and therefore the results presented in
this exploratory
study need to be replicated with larger samples and more age
groups. In
addition, reaction time studies as well as use of qualitative
methods, such as
interviews, think-aloud protocols and eye-movement tracking
could be used to
further validate the VAST.
Although
performance in the VAST differentiates teachers from experts and
is related to
geometry expertise, the results do not provide information about
how analytic
reasoning in geometry actually develops.
Developmental research is needed to further examine the
hypotheses of
the FT that knowledge acquisition in geometry is a continuous
and not a
stage-like process, during which fragmentation and synthetic
conceptions are
formed.
Future
research needs to also investigate the contribution of
intellectual abilities
and mathematics knowledge to VAST performance by administering
additional
tests, such as a propositional test measuring verbal analytical
skills,
measures of visuo-spatial and phonological memory, speed of
processing and
executive function, as well as measures of mathematics
abilities. Results from
these tests could be used as co-variates in order to reduce as
much as possible
the effect of individual differences.
Last
but not least, the relation between individual differences in
spatial thinking
and the use of visual and analytic strategies in geometry needs
to be
investigated further, preferably using longitudinal designs. If,
as we claim,
geometry expertise (and possibly expertise in other domains of
STEM) requires
the eventual development of analytic strategies, why are
individuals who are
good in spatial thinking more successful in STEM disciplines
than those who are
not? A possible explanation of this finding may be that
individuals who are
good in spatial thinking find it easier to do well in geometry
early on, before
conceptual changes in this domain require the development of
analytic
strategies based on formal, geometrical knowledge. Maybe because
of these early
successes, these students develop an interest in geometry (or
other
STEM-related disciplines), spend more time studying, and thus
eventually
undergo the conceptual changes and develop the analytic
strategies required for
geometrical expertise. This conjecture is consistent with the
findings of the
present study that spatial thinking as measured by visuo-spatial
and abstract
(spatial) reasoning tests contributes to success in the VAST,
but is rendered
insignificant for the VAST-InconS when geometrical knowledge is
taken into
account. This issue needs to be further researched.
Keypoints
Considerable
conceptual changes are required to go
from visuo-spatial reasoning to analytic strategies in
geometry
These changes are
related to geometry expertise and
not to individual differences
Questions arise about
the role of visual-spatial
reasoning in geometry expertise
Acknowledgments
The research reported in this paper is supported
by
a grant from the Greek Ministry of Education, General
Secretariat for Research
and Technology, MolVisEdu, THALIS – Aristotle University
of
Thessaloniki. We would like to thank Petros
Roussos for helpful comments.
References
Battista, M. T. (2007).
The
development of geometric and spatial thinking. In F. Lester
(Ed.), Second Handbook of
Research on Mathematics Teaching
and Learning,
NCTM
(pp. 843-908). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.
Bodner, G.M., &
Guay,
R.B. (1997). The Purdue Visualization of Rotations Test. The
Chemical
Educator, 2(4), 1-17.
Burger,
W.
F., & Shaughnessy, J. M. (1986). Characterizing
the van
Hiele levels of
development in geometry. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 17,
31-48.
CSGT
California
Standards Test Geometry sample (2009). http://www.cde.ca.gov/Ta/tg/sr/
documents/cstrtqgeomapr15.pdf
Carey, S. (2009). The
origin
of concepts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cheng,
Y.L., & Mix, K.S. (2012). Spatial
Training Improves Children's Mathematics Ability. Journal of Cognition and
Development
(published online, September 19, 2012). http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/15248372.2012.725186
Chi, M. T. H., (2008).
Three
Types of Conceptual Change: Belief Revision, Mental Model
Transformation, and
Categorical Shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International
Handbook of Research
on Conceptual Change (pp. 61-82). New York, NY:
Routledge.
Clement, J. (1982).
Students'
preconceptions in introductory mechanics. The American
Journal of Physics,
50(1), 66-71.
Clements,
D.
H., & Battista, M. T. (2001). Logo and Geometry.
Journal for Research
in Mathematics Education Monograph.Reston, VA: National
Council of Teachers
of Mathematics.
diSessa,
A.,
(1982). Unlearning Aristotelian physics: A study of
knowledge-based
learning. Cognitive Science, 6(1), 37-75.DOI: 10.1207/s15516709cog0601_2
Eisenberg,
T.,
& Dreyfus, T. (1991). On
the reluctance to visualize in mathematics. In W. Zimmermann
& S.
Cunningham (Eds.), Visualization in teaching and learning
mathematics (pp.
26-37). Washington, DC: MAA.
Evans, J.
St. B.T.,
& Stanovich, K. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher
cognition:
Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 8(3),
223–241. Doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685
Fischbein,
E.
(1993). The theory of figural concepts. Educational Studies
in Mathematics,
24 (2), 139-162.
Gutiérrez, A.,
& Jaime, A.
(1998). On the assessment of the van Hiele levels of
reasoning. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics,
20(2-3),
27-46.
Hegarty, M. (1992).
Mental
animation: Inferring motion from static diagrams of mechanical
systems. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition,
18, 1084–1102.
Hegarty, M., Montello,
D. R.,
Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006).
Spatial Abilities
at Different Scales: Individual Differences in Aptitude-Test
Performance and
Spatial-Layout Learning. Intelligence, 34, 151-176.
Hegarty, M., &
Waller, D.
(2006). Individual differences in spatial abilities. In P.
Shah & A. Miyake
(Eds.), Handbook of Visualspatial Thinking. Cambridge,
MA: Cambridge
University Press.
Houdement, C., &
Kuzniak,
A. (2003). Elementary Geometry Split into Different
Geometrical Paradigms. Proceedings
of CERME 3, Belaria, Italy. http://www.dm.unipi.it/~didattica/
CERME3/proceedings/Groups/TG7/TG7_Houdement_cerme3.pdf
Kastens,
K.A.,
& Ishikawa, T. (2006). Spatial
thinking in the geosciences and cognitive sciences: A
cross-disciplinary look
at the intersection of the two fields. In C. A. Manduca &
D. W. Mogk
(Eds.), Earth and mind: How geologists think and learn
about the Earth (pp.
53-76). Boulder, CO: Geological Society of America.
Kilpatrick, J., Hoyles,
C.,
& Skovsmose, O. (Eds.) in collaboration with Valero, P.
(2005). Meaning
in Mathematics Education, USA: Springer.
Kozhevnikov, M., Motes,
M.,
& Hegarty, M. (2007). Spatial visualization in physics
problem solving. Cognitive
Science, 31, 549-579.
Lehrer,
R.,
Jenkins, M., & Osana. H. (1998). Longitudinal study of
children’s reasoning
about space and geometry. In R. Lehrer & D. Chazan (Eds.),
Designing
learning environments for developing understanding of
geometry and space (pp.
137-167). Mahwah,
NJ:
Erlbaum.
Lynn,
B.
M., & Lynch, C. M. (2010). van Hiele Revisited. Mathematics Teaching In The
Middle School, 16(4), 232-238.
Newcombe,
N.
S. (2010). Picture
this: Increasing math and
science learning by improving spatial thinking.
American
Educator, Summer, 29-43.
Newcombe,
N.
S., & Frick, A. (2010), Early education for spatial
intelligence: Why,
what, and How. Mind, Brain and Education, 4(3),
102-111.
Newcombe, N. S., &
Huttenlocher J. (2000). Making
space: The development of spatial representation and
reasoning. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Newcombe, N. S., Uttal,
D. H.
& Sauter, M. (in press). Spatial Development. In
P. Zelazo (Ed), Oxford
handbook of developmental psychology. New York, NY:
Oxford University
Press.
Piaget, J., &
Inhleder,
B., (1948/1967). The
child’s conception
of space. London:
W.W. Norton &
Company.
Piaget,
J.,
Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1948/1960). The child’s conception of
geometry. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Pitta-Pantazi,
D.,
& Christou, C. (2009). Cognitive styles,
dynamic geometry and measurement
performance. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70,
5-26.
Sanchez,
C.A.
(2012). Enhancing visuospatial performance through video game
training to
increase learning in visuospatial science domains. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
19 (1),
58–65.
Schwartz,
D.L.,
& Black, J.B. (1996). Shuttling between depictive models
and abstract
rules: Induction and fallback. Cognitive Science, 20,
457-497.
Shea, D. L., Lubinski,
D.,
& Benbow, C. P. (2001). Importance of assessing spatial
ability in
intellectually talented young adolescents: A 20-year
longitudinal study. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 604–614.
Shtulman,
A.,
& Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses
but does not
supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124, 209–215.
Spelke, E. S., &
Kinzler,
K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10,
89–96.
Spelke, E.S., Lee, S. A., &
Izard, V. (2010). Beyond core knowledge: Natural
geometry. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 863-884
Stieff,
M.
(2007). Mental rotation and diagrammatic reasoning in science.
Learning
and Instruction, 17, 219-234.
Usiskin, Z. (1982). Van
Hiele
levels and achievement in secondary school geometry.
Colombus, OH:
ERIC.
Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N.
G.,
Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A.R., Warren, C., &
Newcombe, N.S. (2013).
The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of
training studies. Psychological
Bulletin, 139, 352-402.
Van Hiele P. M. (1986).
Structure and
Insight: A theory of Mathematics Education.
London: Academic Press Inc.
Vosniadou,
S.,
& Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental Models of the Day/Night
Cycle. Cognitive
Science, 18, 123-183.
Vosniadou,
S.
(2013). Conceptual change in learning and instruction:
The framework
theory approach. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International
Handbook of Research
on Conceptual Change, 2nd Edition (pp.
11-30).
New York, NY:
Routledge.
Vosniadou, S., &
Skopeliti, I. (2014). Conceptual change from the framework
theory side of the
fence. Science and Education, 23(7), 1427-1445.
Vosniadou, S., Pnevmantikos, D., Makris, N.,
Ikospentaki, K., Lepenioti, D.,
Chountala, A., & Kyrianakis, G. (2015). Executive Functions
and Conceptual Change in Science
and Mathematics Learning, 7th Annual Conference of
the Cognitive
Science Society, Pasadena, CA.
https://mindmodeling.org/cogsci2015/papers/0434/paper0434.pdf
Wai,
J.,
Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM
domains: Aligning over 50
years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its
importance. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817–835.
Wiser, M., & Smith,
C.
(2013). Learning and teaching about matter in the middle
school years. How can
atomic-molecular theory be meaningfully introduced? In S.
Vosniadou (Ed.), International
Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change, 2nd Edition (pp.
177-194).
New York, NY:
Routledge.
Zazkis,
R.,
Dubinsky, E., & Dautermann, J. (1996). Coordinating visual and
analytic strategies: A study
of students' understanding of the group D 4. Journal for
Research in
Mathematics Education, 27(4), 435–457.
[1] There is a great deal of research on spatial
development in young children
and different theoretical approaches have appeared after
Piaget’s seminal work
(see Newcombe & Huttenlocher, 2000;
Spelke & Kinzler, 2007) but this
is not the focus of the present paper.
41
[2] Various theories are attempting to explain
early spatial
development including connectionist interpretations and
neoconstructivist
approaches (see Newcombe, Uttal, & Sauer, in press,
for an extensive
review).
[3] Synthetic
conceptions
are formed when learners assimilate scientific information
to their
incompatible prior knowledge producing in the process an
alternative, erroneous
conception, which however has some internal consistency
and explanatory value,
such as the ‘impetus misconception’ in mechanics (Clement,
1982), the
‘molecules in matter’ model in the atomic-molecular theory
(Wiser & Smith,
2013), and the ‘hollow sphere’ model in observational
astronomy (Vosniadou,
2013; Vosniadou & Brewer 1994).
In geometry, the ‘figural object’ described by
Fischbein (1993) to be
formed from the synthesis of the ‘perceptual’ and
‘conceptual’ objects
described by Batista (2007) is such a hybrid, synthetic
conception.
[4] The
students had been taught the basic geometric concepts and
methods based on
empirical measurements and inductive generalizations in
grades 7, 8, and 9. In
grades 10 and 11 they were introduced to the procedures of
deductive proofs
characterizing Euclidean geometry.