Volition completes the puzzle:
Development and
evaluation of an integrative trait model of self-regulated
learning
Laura
Dörrenbächer[1], Franziska Perels
Department of Educational
Sciences, Saarland
University
Article received 20
May / revised 25 October
/ accepted 26
October / available online 15 December
Abstract
Most
self-regulated
learning theories are imbedded within a social-cognitive
framework and comprise cognitive, metacognitive and
motivational components.
Nevertheless, these theories partly neglect volition, which
is necessary for
implementing learning intentions. Therefore, the present
study is frontline as
it aimed to integrate volition within a comprehensive trait
model of
self-regulated learning (SRL) while proposing a new
conception of trait
volition for learning. A sample of n = 377 college students
(70.1% female, MAge
= 23.36, SDAge = 4.12) filled out questionnaires
concerning
volitional, cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational
belief aspects of SRL.
The results of confirmatory factor analysis speak in favour
of integrating the
highly interrelated constructs of procrastination, future
time perspective, and
academic delay of gratification in order to depict volition
for SRL. Moreover,
the structural equation modelling results favour a twofold
motivational
component for SRL that comprises both motivational beliefs
and volition instead
of including volition as a separate component aside from
cognitive,
metacognitive and motivational belief components.
Additionally, the comprehensive
trait model of SRL is related to GPA, which is a first
indication of its
validity. Therefore, the study empirically investigates a
new conception of
trait volition for learning environments as well as its
integration within a
comprehensive SRL framework. Future research should consider
the importance of
volitional components for SRL and could investigate
individual differences
concerning the modelled components.
Keywords: self-regulated
learning; volition; academic delay of gratification;
procrastination; future time
perspective
[1]
Corresponding
author: Laura Dörrenbächer, Department of Educational
Sciences, Saarland
University, Campus Building A 4 2, 66123 Saarbrücken, Germany.
Phone: +49(0)681
/ 30258337, Fax: +49(0)681/ 30258341, Email: laura.doerrenbaecher@uni-saarland.de
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.14786/flr.v3i4.179
1.
Introduction
Although there are many different
models
to explain self-regulated learning (hereinafter referred to as
SRL), they all
characterize the learner as an individual that is
self-determined and that
actively creates his or her learning process (Efklides, 2011).
A self-regulated
learner therefore has the ability to set goals and to
accomplish these goals by
monitoring, controlling, and altering his or her behaviour,
motivation and
cognition adaptively as a response to changing environmental
factors (Pintrich,
2000; Zimmerman, 2000). Consistently, most authors agree that
SRL embraces
cognitive, metacognitive as well as motivational components
that interact
reciprocally (Boekaerts, 1999). Recent research has shown that
SRL
positively influences academic
outcomes in different areas of education (e.g. Dignath,
Büttner, &
Langfeldt, 2008; Kitsantas, Winsler, & Huie, 2008) and
that college
students show better performance when self-regulative
strategies are used
(Nandagopal & Ericsson, 2012). Concerning its conceptual
status, SRL can be
seen as a trait that influences individual learning processes
on a general
level (e.g. Boekaerts, 1999) or as a dynamic state that
changes cyclically
according to situational demands (e.g. Schmitz & Wiese,
2006). Recently,
there have also been models proposed that integrate both
conceptual dimensions
(e.g. Efklides, 2011) because SRL can be regarded as an
aptitude and an event
(Winne & Perry, 2000). Matthews, Schwean, Campbell,
Saklofske, and Mohamed
(2000) concordantly argue that SRL has nomothetic (trait) as
well as
idiographic (state) qualities. As states are influenced by
corresponding traits
(Hong, 1995) and traits help to explain individual differences
(Hong &
O’Neil, 2001), the present study aims to develop and evaluate
an integrative
trait model of SRL that could be useful for future research.
The trait model of
Hong & O’Neil (2001) is used as a basis as it already has
been tested
empirically. We investigate an extended model that also
includes a cognitive
component (Boekaerts, 1999) and considers several
subcomponents of
metacognition and motivation that are important for depicting
SRL
comprehensively.
Even though the definition of
self-regulated students as “metacognitively, motivationally,
and behaviourally
active participants in their own learning process” (Zimmerman,
2008, p. 167)
takes into account volitional aspects, SRL research largely
has underemphasized
such abilities that can predict academic achievement as well
(Duckworth,
Gendler, & Gross, 2014). Volition is the capability to
inhibit distracting
behaviours in order to attain a higher goal (Duckworth &
Seligman, 2006)
and helps to protect learning intentions from action
tendencies competing with
that goal (Corno, 2001). As it has been mostly described
within action-control
theory (Heckhausen & Kuhl, 1985), several authors demand
for adding
volitional aspects above and beyond motivational, cognitive,
and metacognitive
components within a social-cognitive framework when modelling
SRL (Wolters
& Benzon, 2013; Zimmerman, 2011). Therefore, the present
study examines the
conceptual structure of volition comparing two integrative
trait models of SRL
(see figure 1): One model treats volition as a separate
component of SRL besides
cognitive, metacognitive and motivational belief components
(Corno, 2001), while
the second model categorizes it as a motivational subcomponent
besides
motivational beliefs[2]
(Zimmerman, 2008). In
order to validate the models, their relation to GPA of
university entrance diploma
will be analysed using structural equation modelling. In the
context of
integrating volition into the SRL framework, a new
conceptualization of trait
volition is presented: We chose procrastination, future time
perspective, and
academic delay of gratification as these constructs represent
three volitional
traits that are highly interrelated (e.g. Bembenutty &
Karabenick, 2004;
Sirois, 2014) and that act as important supporters for SRL
(e.g. Park &
Sperling, 2012; Zimmerman, 2011).
Altogether, the present study adds to
research because it evaluates an extended trait model of SRL
that takes into
account cognitive and metacognitive components as well as
motivational beliefs
and volition while the position of volition within the SRL
framework is
examined. In this context, a new conceptualization of volition
for learning
environments is developed and tested. The present study is
frontline as it
brings together two highly relevant theoretical frameworks for
the field of
educational psychology whose relation has been neglected for a
long time. After
presenting the basis for our comprehensive SRL trait model, we
will describe
the research lines of SRL and volition in order to point out
differences
between them. In a next step, we present attempts to integrate
both frameworks
and depict the conceptualization of trait volition for SRL.
1.1
Trait-conception of SRL
Traits represent relatively stable
characteristics that influence and predict performance across
a wide range of
tasks (Hertzog & Nesselroade, 2003). Thus, trait SRL can
be described as a
general disposition of students and learners in general
(Boekaerts & Corno,
2005) or as relatively stable tendencies to use SRL
strategies. Individuals
therefore will respond relatively consistently to a range of
different learning
situations: Learners with high SRL trait values should show
more metacognitive
skills and be more self-efficacious than should individuals
with low values on
this trait (Hong & O’Neil, 2001). The fact that SRL is
related to other
personality traits and achievement motives also speaks in
favour of the trait
perspective (Wolters & Hussain, 2014).
Although the concept of state SRL has
recently gained importance in literature and stimulated a
great deal of
research (Azevedo, 2014), the examination of trait SRL is
necessary to
understand individual differences in SRL and achievement.
Accordingly, several
studies have shown the positive influence of SRL on academic
outcomes in all areas
of education (e.g. Dignath, Büttner, & Langfeldt, 2008;
Kitsantas, Winsler,
& Huie, 2008) and therefore constitute its relevance and
its meaning for
lifelong learning (Bronson, 2000). In line with this, college
students show
better test performance when self-regulative strategies are
used during test
preparation and accomplishment (Kitsantas, 2002), and their
grade point average
(GPA) level differs depending on the use of self-regulative
learning strategies
(Nandagopal & Ericsson, 2012). Moreover, the examination
of SRL as a trait
can help to explain differential effects of intervention
programs (Hong &
O’Neil, 2001). Therefore, it can have practical implications
for dealing with
heterogeneity in learning and in fostering learning
competences.
The trait SRL model of Hong
& O’Neil (2001) can be used as a starting point for the
development of an
integrative model. They conceptualize SRL as a third-order
factor that subsumes
the two second-order factors metacognition and motivation,
which are highly
relevant components of SRL and which most models have in
common (Efklides,
2011). The metacognitive factor comprises the subcomponents of
planning one’s
time and strategy use as well as the construct of
self-checking, which is a
method to control proceedings and adapt learning behaviour in
a goal-oriented
way. The conceptualization of the motivational factor, which
is represented by
effort as well as self-efficacy, indicates a combination of
two motivational
concepts: Whereas self-efficacy refers to more or less
unconscious beliefs or
attitudes about one’s skills and competences (Bandura, 1997),
effort comprises
processes that include deliberate thoughts or behaviours in
order to reach a
goal (Carver & Scheier, 2000). Therefore, self-efficacy as
a motivational
belief mostly influences goal-setting processes, whereas
effort is important for
the initiation and implementation of an action (Corno, 2001).
Effort therefore
can be considered as a volitional component of SRL, helping
students to focus attention
and to deal with distractions of personal and environmental
origin (Zimmerman,
2011). Even though volition can have the role of a mediator
between learning
intentions and the actual use of learning strategies, models
of SRL mostly have
overlooked volitional components (Garcia, McCann, Turner,
& Roska, 1998).
Although the previously described
model (Hong & O’Neil, 2001) integrates various important
components of SRL,
it has several points of criticism: At first, the motivational
belief component
is only represented by self-efficacy, although goal
orientations, intrinsic
motivation, or causal attributions are also crucial
motivational properties
(Zimmerman, 2008). Moreover, it simplifies the volitional
component by reducing
it to effort and therefore neglects other important volitional
factors.
Additionally, the model lacks the integration of a
second-order cognitive
factor or the use of cognitive learning strategies that
represent a further
important SRL component (e.g. organization, critical thinking;
Pintrich, 2000).
As the metacognitive factor comprises the construct of
self-checking, it blends
the capabilities of self-recording as an observational method
and
self-evaluation as a judgment of one’s own actions although
these are located
in different phases of a self-regulated learning cycle
(Zimmerman, 2000). Concluding,
Hong and O’Neil’s model (2001) is a first attempt to integrate
several
important components of SRL and to test this structure
empirically but has
several shortcomings. Motivated by the abovementioned points
of criticism, the
present study adds to research as a cognitive factor is
included, the
metacognitive and motivational belief factors are extended by
adding several
subcomponents and the structural position of the volition
factor is examined in
more detail. Therefore, the next sections aim to theoretically
integrate trait
volition within a broader framework of SRL considering the
previously described
shortcomings and to propose an extended concept of trait
volition with regard
to SRL. As far as we know, this study is the first to bring
together these
research lines and to test such an integrative SRL trait model
empirically.
1.2
Integration of volition into SRL
1.2.1
Linking
two frameworks
Self-regulated learning is
described as the ability to set goals that are accomplished by
monitoring,
controlling, and altering one’s behaviour, motivation and
cognition in response
to environmental conditions that are continuously changing
(Zimmerman, 2000).
This interaction of personal, behavioural and environmental
processes reflects
a social-cognitive perspective for describing learning
processes (Bandura,
1986). Feedback loops between personal and environmental
factors are assumed
and represent their interdependence. Accordingly, Zimmerman’s
process model of
self-regulated learning (2000) distinguishes between
forethought, performance
and self-reflection phases that comprise these interacting
factors. The learner
adapts his or her thoughts, affects and behaviour cyclically
in order to attain
a previous set goal. The social-cognitive framework therefore
underlines the
role of the learner’s context and situation when goals are set
as well as of previous
performance when expectations are formed (Zimmerman &
Schunk, 2001).
Volition is defined as the
capability to inhibit irrelevant behaviours in order to attain
a higher goal
(Duckworth & Seligman, 2006). Concerning learning
environments, volition helps
to protect learning intentions in the presence of competing
action tendencies or
obstacles (Corno, 2001) by the use of action- control
strategies (Kuhl, 2000).
Therefore, volition is of importance when students have to
maintain their
concentration and effort in the presence of internal or
external distractions because
it supports tenacity (Zimmerman, 2011). The construct has
mostly been described
within action-control theory (Kuhl, 1984), where it is seen as
a mediator
between the intention to learn and the actual use of learning
strategies
(Corno, 1993). Concerning this framework, goal-directed
activities can be
divided into two distinct phases (Heckhausen & Kuhl,
1985): The pre-decisional
phase comprises motivational processes that entail the choice
of a specific
goal as well as the appraisal of the goal’s value and
intention formation. The
subsequent post-decisional phase involves the implementation
of goal-directed
behaviours by the use of volitional strategies that help to
maintain one’s
intentions (Corno, 2001; Garcia et al., 1998). In this
context, the framework
describes how motivation and volition are interwoven and hints
at the
assumption that volition is a part of a broader motivational
concept: Choice
motivation influences goal setting and expectancy-value
processes in the
pre-decisional phase and therefore represents motivational
beliefs like
self-efficacy, task value and goal orientations (Husman,
McCann & Crowson,
2000). Executive motivation however takes place in the
post-decisional phase
and affects action implementation and effort maintenance.
According to
Gollwitzer (1996), choice motivation is characterized by a
motivational
mindset, whereas a volitional mindset comes along with
executive motivation.
As most SRL theories are
imbedded within a social-cognitive framework (Zimmerman,
2000), motivational beliefs
within the predecisional phase (e.g. self-efficacy, goal
orientation) have been
of particular interest for this line of research. Concerning
the postdecisional
phase of learning processes, SRL models focus on the
application of cognitive
learning strategies and therefore partly neglect volitional
strategies
necessary to implement one’s intentions (Garcia et al., 1998).
As Garcia et al.
(1998) put it, executive motivation has mostly been neglected
within SRL
frameworks and the majority of SRL models propose that
processes of choice
motivation “complete the puzzle” (p. 396). Hence, there is a
lack of studies that
investigate the relation of volition to SRL. Accordingly,
several authors speak
in favour of integrating volition within models of SRL (e.g.
Duckworth et al.;
2014, Zimmerman, 2008) in addition to aspects of cognition,
metacognition, and
motivation. Concordantly, Corno (2001) argues that volition
complements motivation
and that both concepts taken together represent an action
disposition.
Nevertheless, action-control theory assumes a relatively
absolute distinction
between both concepts as choice motivation is terminated when
the pre-actional
phase and volitional processes start (Heckhausen & Kuhl,
1985). A
social-cognitive view would be more dynamic, assuming an
interaction between choice
and executive motivational processes (Wolters, 2003b) and
would question if
volition is separable from traditional motivational measures
like goal
expectations (Zimmerman & Schunk, 2001). Therefore, a
cross-fertilization
of the conceptions could be beneficial for both
social-cognitive and
action-control theory (Duckworth et al., 2014). The
abovementioned twofold
conception of motivation (self-efficacy and effort) in Hong
and O’Neil’s model
(2001) is a first attempt to accommodate for this demand.
Moreover, a recent
study found that the construct of self-discipline, which is an
expression of executive
motivation and therefore stems from action-control theory, and
SRL, which is a
social-cognitive construct, were highly interrelated
(Zimmerman &
Kitsantas, 2014). Nevertheless, SRL had a significantly higher
predictive
validity for GPA than did self-discipline, and a two-factor
model solution
provided better fit indices in this study sample.
One attempt to integrate
motivation and volition within a social-cognitive framework
came from Corno and
Kanfer (1993) who developed a model that illustrates the role
of volition in
the context of learning and motivation. Besides intrinsic and
extrinsic
motivation, volitional styles, action control and goal-related
cognitions are
integrated within the model. Nevertheless, this model has, as
far as we know, not
been empirically tested yet and it neglects metacognitive
components which
represent a key factor of self-regulated learning. Moreover,
by integrating a
strict distinction between decision-making and action
implementation, the model
is relatively rigid and neglects possible interactions between
the two
components. Another
research line
accounting for volitional aspects of learning that is imbedded
within the
social-cognitive framework of SRL focuses on the regulation of
motivation and
speaks in favour of subsuming motivational beliefs and
volition under the
broader term of motivation. The construct of motivation
regulation is
volitional in nature and comprises actions that support the
purposeful
initiation and maintenance of goal-directed behaviour
(Wolters, 2003b).
Therefore, it is a critical aspect of SRL that must coalesce
with motivational
beliefs and metacognitive processes to ensure success when
learning. This
importance speaks in favour of integrating it into the broader
system of SRL
(Wolters, 1999, 2003b). Wolters and Benzon (2013) have argued
that empirical
work concerning the link between regulation of motivation and
other dimensions
of SRL has to be extended, and that it has to be examined how
this relation could
be modelled at a general level (Wolters, 2003b). Nevertheless,
motivational
regulation only affects one category of volitional control and
therefore does
not depict the whole volitional framework (Kuhl, 1985).
Moreover, although
volition seems to support the influence of motivational
processes on cognitive
effort (Garcia et al., 1998), Husman et al. (2000) argue that
research has not
yet examined the relationship of motivational processes and
volition. Concluding,
the existing literature speaks in favour of integrating
volition within models
of SRL. As volition has mostly been neglected in SRL research,
we will present
a new conceptualization of volition for SRL by integrating
academic delay of
gratification, future time perspective and procrastination.
1.2.2
An
extended conceptualization of trait volition for SRL
As with SRL, volitional traits
can be distinguished from volitional states: It is argued that
the use of
volitional strategies may be an indicator of a dispositional
trait ability to
reach goals by controlling distractions (Corno, 1994).
Volitional styles
represent dispositional tendencies that influence goal
implementation and are
relatively stable (Corno & Kanfer, 1993). Kuhl’s (1985)
differentiation of
action- vs. state-orientation is an example for treating
volitional styles as a
predisposition that influences action. Concordantly, Boekaerts
and Corno (2005)
designated those strategies as habits supporting an effective
working style,
suggesting a trait conception. In order to conceptualize a
volitional trait in
the framework of SRL, we integrate academic delay of
gratification,
procrastination, and future time perspective. These constructs
have been
selected because they represent volition within learning
environments (e.g.
Steel, 2007), show a relatively high stability (e.g. Sirois,
2014), are
considered as SRL features (e.g. Bembenutty & Karabenick,
2004), and are
highly interrelated (e.g. Dewitte & Lens, 2000). Moreover,
several authors
have argued for their investigation within an SRL framework
(Bembenutty &
Karabenick, 2004).
Academic delay of
gratification is defined as postponing proximate, impulse
satisfying actions to
sustain previously intended actions oriented towards a distant
but apparently
more valuable academic goal (Bembenutty, 2008), and is
therefore volitional in
nature (Bembenutty & Karabenick, 2004). Depending on the
underlying
theoretical model, academic delay of gratification can be seen
as “a volitional
strategy, a cognitive schema, a general disposition or a
personal trait”
(Pintrich, 1999, p. 346). Since it is mostly described as the
ability to wait
for temporarily distant rewards (Bembenutty, 2008), the
construct is seen as a
trait in the present study. Moreover, several studies speak in
favour of the
construct’s relevance for academic achievement (e.g. di
Benedetto &
Bembenutty, 2013). It has been shown that this trait is
associated with the
level of self-regulation as well as the use of SRL strategies,
and can be
embedded within a broader framework of SRL (Bembenutty, 2008,
2009; Bembenutty
& Karabenick, 2004).
Contradictory to this
construct, procrastination is defined as a deliberate delay of
intended actions
although this delay in all probability has negative effects on
reaching an
important goal (Steel, 2007). It is therefore the opposite of
motivated and
volitional behaviour (Keller, 2008), and demonstrates a lack
of effort
regulation (Rakes & Dunn, 2010) or a volitional breakdown
(Sirois, 2014).
Concerning its conceptual status, most authors conclude that
procrastination
results from a trait-like tendency in behaviour (Sirois,
2014), with high
stability over a period of ten years (Steel, 2007).
Procrastination is a highly
relevant construct in academic learning settings because it is
very common
among students (Schouwenburg, 2004) and negatively related to
academic
achievement (Akinsola, Tella, & Tella, 2007). For the
integration of
procrastination within an SRL framework, it could be argued
that the two concepts
may be opposite ends on the same regulatory continuum (Dietz,
Hofer, &
Fries, 2007; Park & Sperling, 2012), and that the level of
procrastination
is negatively related to metacognitive strategies (Howell
& Watson, 2007;
Wolters, 2003a).
The concept of future time
perspective is represented by a conceptualization of time that
is directed to
the future and entails future-oriented beliefs concerning
specific life domains
(Peetsma, Schuitema, & van der Veen, 2012). The construct
supports volition
because it has a positive influence on the maintenance of
motivation (Dewitte
& Lens, 2000) and intention implementation (de Bilde,
Vansteenkiste, &
Lens, 2011). It is therefore related to the concept of
maintenance
self-efficacy (Luszczynska & Sutton, 2006) that is
important for
persistence on tasks. Future time perspective moreover fosters
an inner pressure
to achieve goals and is therefore a form of volitional
motivation. Although it
can be seen as a cognitive-motivational concept, the perceived
instrumentality
of future goals causes a stronger effort in learning (Simons,
Dewitte &
Lens, 2004), why we consider future time perspective as
volitional. Similar to
academic delay of gratification and procrastination, it is
conceptualized as a general
predisposition that is stable over time (Peetsma et al., 2012;
Zimbardo &
Boyd, 1999). Students with this cognitive temporal bias tend
to show better
study outcomes (Horstmanshof & Zimitat, 2007) and are more
conscientious
(Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999). Several authors have stated that
a time
perspective directed towards the future has an important
influence on motivational
processes and the use of SRL strategies (de Bilde et al.,
2011; Miller &
Brickman, 2004; Zimmerman, 2011).
Several results speak in favour
of a high interrelation of these constructs, and therefore
support the
hypothesis that they represent volition. A meta-analysis of 14
samples showed
that students with a future time perspective displayed less
procrastination
(Sirois, 2014), and the fact that procrastinators are unable
to postpone
gratification expresses low levels of academic delay of
gratification and low
inhibitory control (Tuckman, 1991). Moreover, some authors
have argued that
future time perspective is responsible for the conceptual
relationship of
procrastination and academic delay of gratification (Dewitte
& Lens, 2000)
and that it is necessary to delay gratifications in academic
settings
(Bembenutty & Karabenick, 2004). Concluding, it seems
reasonable to
integrate these three constructs in order to depict volition
within a SRL
framework.
1.3
Purpose of the present study
As abovementioned, the present
study aims to investigate several theoretical aspects
concerning the
conceptualization of volition within SRL: The overall aim is
the development
and evaluation of an integrative SRL trait model that takes
into account the
previously addressed shortcomings of the Hong and O’Neil model
(2001). Besides
the integration of a cognitive factor, the proposed model
extends the
metacognitive and motivational belief component and includes a
volitional
factor. Accompanying this extension, the comparison of two
integrative SRL
trait models should clarify the role of the volitional factor:
One model
integrates volition as a separate aspect distinct from
cognition, metacognition
and motivational beliefs (Corno, 2001), whereas the other one
specifies the
motivational component by integrating motivational beliefs and
volition (Garcia
et al., 1998; Wolters, 2003b; Zimmerman, 2008; see figure 1).
Moreover, a new
conception of trait volition for SRL that integrates academic
delay of
gratification, procrastination, and future time perspective is
presented and
evaluated. In order to validate the models, their relation to
GPA of university
entrance diploma will be analysed using structural equation
modelling because SRL
is associated with academic achievement (Dignath et al.,
2008). Altogether, the
present study adds to research by examining the structural
relationship of
academic delay of gratification, future time perspective and
procrastination
concertedly within an integrative trait model of SRL that
encompasses
cognitive, metacognitive, motivational belief as well as
volitional components.
It is frontline as it brings together two highly relevant
educational concepts
whose relation has been analysed rarely.
Figure
1. Two
proposed integrative SRL models: Model 1 includes volition
(VOL) as a factor
besides cognition (COG), metacognition (META) and motivational
beliefs (MB)
while model 2 extends the motivational factor (MOT) by
including volition
besides motivational beliefs.
2.
Method
2.1
Sample and sampling procedure
The sample comprised N = 381 undergraduate
students from a
southwestern German university. As four participants had
missing data on all
variables, they were excluded from the following analyses.
Therefore, a total
sample of n = 377
(70.1% female) with
an age range from 17 to 45 years (M =
23.36, SD = 4.12)
was analysed. The
mean GPA of university entrance diploma was M
= 2.10 (SD = 0.60,
ranging from 1 =
excellent to 4 = poor), indicating that there is no ceiling
effect concerning the
sample’s academic achievement level. The students were
enrolled in very
different fields of study (pre-service teachers of different
subjects [65.0%], psychology
[16.2%], languages and cultural studies [9.8%], natural
sciences [2.9%], economics
and law [2.4%], informatics [1.3%], other/not specified
[2.2%]) and therefore
reflected a large portion of available fields of study in
Germany. The sample
comprised students of all phases of their studies (year one:
25.0%, year two:
16.9%. year three: 25.7%, year four: 16.8%, year five or
higher: 15.7%), while
year of study was no predictor for SRL (t(376)
= -0.24, p = .81).
Testing was
embedded into the first session of several university courses
and students had
the chance to win a shopping voucher. Participants had to sign
an informed
consent as participation was voluntary and data were
anonymised by codes. At
the beginning of the test session, every participant received
an informed
consent form that explained the purpose of the study and the
use of the data gained.
By signing the form, participants agreed to this procedure.
The data collection
was part of a larger project and was conducted with the
lecturers’ permission.
2.2
Instruments
2.2.1
Demographic
information and academic performance
The first part of the
questionnaire recorded demographic information such as gender,
age, field of
study, and GPA of university entrance diploma (ranging from 1
= excellent to 4 = poor). This university entrance diploma is the
result of national school
exams that are curricular-based and therefore comparable
across different
schools and regions. All university students pass this exam in
the same class
level ensuring a comparable educational level. As GPA of
university entrance
diploma is used for applicant selection at many universities
and has a strong
relationship with later university achievement (Wedler,
Troche, &
Rammsayer, 2008), it is very central in the German educational
system.
Moreover, it is comparable between students of all subjects of
study which
would not be the case for GPA of subject of study
(Müller-Benedict &
Tsarouha, 2011).
2.2.2
SRL
inventory
In order to measure students’
SRL trait, a questionnaire consisting of 32 items was
developed. Questionnaires
are appropriate to measure traits as they record generalized
assessments concerning
specific domains and abilities (Veenman, 2011). The items were
adopted from
existing inventories that measure SRL (e.g. Jerusalem &
Schwarzer, 1981;
Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1991; Wild &
Schiefele, 1994) or
have been newly developed in order to optimize several
subscales. The inventory
measured cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational belief
variables, and
therefore represents the most popular categories of SRL traits
(Boekaerts,
1999). The items were rated on a four-point Likert-type
format, ranging from 1
(I don’t agree at all)
to 4 (I totally agree).
Subscales with item
examples, Cronbach’s alphas, and number of items can be seen
in Table 1. The
factor structure of the three SRL components was tested using
confirmatory
factor analysis. The components (cognition, metacognition,
motivational beliefs)
each were modelled as latent second-order factors with their
subscales as latent
first-order factors and items as manifest variables. The
results are acceptable
for motivational beliefs [χ² (50) = 128.61, p
< .01, χ²/df = 2.57,
RMSEA = 0.065
[0.051 – 0.078], SRMR = 0.061, CFI = 0.934], metacognition [χ²
(71) = 194.74, p
< .01, χ²/df = 2.74, RMSEA = 0.068 [0.057 – 0.079],
SRMR = 0.067, CFI =
0.934], and cognition [χ² (8) = 22.31, p
< .01, χ²/df = 2.79,
RMSEA = 0.069
[0.036 – 0.104], SRMR = 0.042, CFI = 0.949].
2.2.3
Volition
inventory
As our study aimed to
integrate three volitional traits (future time perspective,
procrastination,
and academic delay of gratification), a questionnaire
comprising items of these
three constructs was developed. The items stem from existing
inventories to
measure these constructs (Academic Delay of Gratification
Scale, Bembenutty
& Karabenick, 1998; Procrastination Scale, Tuckman, 1991;
Future Scale of
the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory, Zimbardo & Boyd,
1999). The items
of future time perspective and procrastination were rated on a
four-point
Likert-type format, ranging from 1 (I
don’t agree at all) to 4 (I totally
agree). The procrastination items register the presence
of procrastinating
behaviour and therefore present a lack of volition. As the
items of the
academic delay of gratification scale consisted of two action
alternatives, A
and B, that represent the ends of a “delay of
gratification-continuum”, these
were rated on a four-point answer format, ranging from 1 (definitely chose A), 2 (rather
chose A), 3 (rather
chose B), and
4 (definitely chose B).
B represents
the alternative that reflects the highest ability to delay
gratification and
therefore the strongest volitional control. All items should
be answered with
regard to students’ learning behaviour. Subscales with item
examples,
Cronbach’s alphas and number of items can be seen in Table 1.
Table 1
Scales
and subscales of the self-regulated learning and volition
inventory
scale |
subscale |
item example |
Cronbach’s alpha
(number of items) |
metacognition |
planning |
“I write a time
schedule before I start learning.” |
.88 (5) |
self-recording |
“I pay attention to not miss my
goal when I’m learning.” |
.70 (4) |
|
self-evaluation |
“After learning,
I check if I’ve reached my goals.” |
.76 (5) |
|
cognition |
organization |
“I draw charts or diagrams in
order to structure learning materials.” |
.52 (3) |
critical thinking |
“I critically
question things I learn.” |
.70 (3) |
|
motivational
beliefs |
self-efficacy |
“I’m able to find a solution
for every problem.” |
.77 (5) |
intrinsic
motivation |
“I enjoy
learning.” |
.70 (3) |
|
goal orientation |
“I prefer tasks that are
interesting, even if they’re difficult to solve.” |
.68 (4) |
|
volition |
future time
perspective |
“I even work on
difficult and boring tasks, when I know, that they
are important for my future.” |
.74 (4) |
academic delay of gratification |
„I would rather A spend
time with friends shortly before an exam or B learn
each day for the exam and spend less time with
friends.” |
.68 (4) |
|
procrastination |
“If something is
too difficult to start with, I postpone the task.” |
.87 (6) |
2.3
Data analysis
In order to test our
conception of trait volition and the extended model of trait
SRL, we used maximum
likelihood parameter estimation with MPlus7 (Muthén &
Muthén, 2012). Using
confirmatory factor analysis, the factorial structure and the
fit of the
proposed models were estimated. The model fit is assessed by
evaluating the
model’s χ², its RMSEA (root mean square error of
approximation), SRMR
(standardized root mean square residual), and CFI (comparative
fit index). A
good fit is characterized by a non-significant χ² (p > .05). As this test is less reliable with
large sample sizes
(Kline, 2005), one can examine the χ²/df-ratio, which should
be below 2:1 to
mark an acceptable fit (Schermelleh-Engel, Moosbrugger, &
Müller, 2003).
RMSEA and SRMR values should be ≤ 0.08 and the CFI as a
fit-index has to be
> 0.90 to indicate a good fit (Kline, 2005). Besides these
model fit
estimations, we assessed the criterion validity by analyzing
the models’
relation to the GPA of university entrance diplomas using
structural equation
modelling (SEM).
3.
Results
3.1
Initial data screening
Table 2 shows the descriptive
statistics and the bivariate zero-order correlation matrix of
the measured
variables. In a first step, the data were screened in order to
find outliers
and to examine missing data, as well as to assess the
linearity and normality
of the data. Except for the four participants that were
excluded from the
analyses because they had missing values on all variables,
there were no
participants with a lot of missing data or outlier values. For
all variables of
the SRL questionnaire as well as the volition inventory and
the GPA of
university entrance diploma Little’s MCAR test (Little &
Rubin, 2002)
indicated that the missing data in this study occurred
completely at random (χ²
(35) = 29.69, p =
.72). MPlus7 uses
the FIML-estimator (full information maximum likelihood) to
treat missing
values, so we did not impute them. Moreover, the data violated
the assumption
of a normal distribution why the MLR estimator—which is robust
to non-normality—was
used to run the analyses.
Table 2
Bivariate
correlations among variable as well as their means and
standard deviations
|
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
10 |
11 |
12 |
1. GPA |
- |
.16** |
-.17** |
-.23** |
-.08 |
-.18** |
-.22** |
-.16** |
-.17** |
-.15** |
-.13* |
-.16** |
2. procrastination |
|
- |
-.74** |
-.42** |
-.11* |
-.29** |
-.26** |
-.53** |
-.49** |
-.43** |
-.33** |
-.22** |
3. future time
perspective |
|
|
- |
.47** |
.09 |
.26** |
.29** |
.51** |
.54** |
.38** |
.36** |
.25** |
4. academic delay
of gratification |
|
|
|
- |
-.10 |
.28** |
.20** |
.39** |
.30** |
.28** |
.30** |
.13* |
5. self-efficacy |
|
|
|
|
- |
.26** |
.43** |
.05 |
.12* |
.01 |
.06 |
.17** |
6. intrinsic
motivation |
|
|
|
|
|
- |
.49** |
.10 |
.20** |
.16** |
.23** |
.35** |
7. goal-orientation |
|
|
|
|
|
|
- |
.18** |
.27** |
.18** |
.21** |
.35** |
8. planning |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
- |
.50** |
.44** |
.37** |
.12* |
9. self-recording |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
- |
.54** |
.45** |
.42** |
10. self-evaluation |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
- |
.23** |
.26** |
11. organization |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
- |
.28** |
12. critical
thinking |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
- |
M |
2.10 |
2.60 |
2.78 |
3.01 |
2.81 |
3.05 |
3.05 |
2.44 |
2.85 |
2.60 |
2.77 |
2.44 |
SD |
0.60 |
0.72 |
0.57 |
0.63 |
0.54 |
0.56 |
0.51 |
0.75 |
0.48 |
0.57 |
0.61 |
0.60 |
Note.
313 ≤ n ≤ 377,
* p < .05, ** p < .01
3.2
Confirmatory factor analysis of trait
volition
In order to examine the factor
structure of the 14 items of the volition inventory, a latent
model of volition
with three latent first-order factors (procrastination,
academic delay of
gratification, future time perspective) and one latent
second-order factor
(volition) using confirmatory factor analyses was tested. The
results speak in
favour of a good model fit: χ² (75) = 123.12, p < .01, χ²/df = 1.64,
RMSEA = 0.041 [0.028 – 0.054], SRMR = 0.039, CFI = 0.971.
Figure 2 shows the
model of trait volition with standardized factor loadings that
are all
significant (all p values
< .001).
The first-order procrastination factor has a negative loading
as the
procrastination items register the presence of procrastinating
behaviour and
therefore present a lack of volition.
Figure
2. Trait
model of volition for SRL with
standardized coefficients. VOL
volition, FTP
future time
perspective, PRO procrastination,
ADOG academic delay
of gratification.
All factor loadings are significant (p
< .001).
3.3
Testing the hypothesized model
In a second step, we tested
the comprehensive trait model of SRL by including the
volitional factor besides
cognitive, metacognitive and motivational belief components
into the model of
SRL. As the internal consistencies of the SRL inventory show
acceptable to
satisfying values and the factor structure of the volition
inventory was
confirmed, the latent factors were estimated by using the
respective subscales
as observed variables. Because we wanted to examine whether
volition—as
we conceptualize it—is
a fourth factor of SRL
(model 1) or is a part of motivation in addition to
motivational beliefs (model
2), we compared two models. Table 3 shows the fit-indices of
both models,
indicating that the model with volition as a subcomponent of
motivation fits
the data more adequately (model 2). Moreover, as the model
alternatives were
constructed based on theoretical assumptions, we used the Bayesian information
criterion to compare the
two models (Burnham & Anderson, 2004). Model 2 has the
lower BIC value and
therefore seems more appropriate to model the data (Geiser,
2011, see table 3).
Table 3
Fit-indices of the
compared SRL-models
model |
χ² |
df |
χ²/df |
RMSEA |
SRMR |
CFI |
BIC |
1 |
54.13 |
32 |
1.69 |
0.043 [0.022 – 0.062] |
0.040 |
0.980 |
6287.76 |
2 |
45.11 |
31 |
1.46 |
0.035 [0.003 – 0.056] |
0.035 |
0.987 |
6283.48 |
3.4
Testing the criterion validity of SRL for
achievement
In order to test the relation
of our integrative SRL model with academic achievement, we
included GPA of the
university entrance diploma as a manifest variable into the
structural model.
Although this measure is not really predictive because it was
obtained in the
past, we had to take this GPA instead of current GPA of
university subject because
it is more comparable between students of different subjects
of study
(Müller-Benedict & Tsarouha, 2011). Additionally, GPA of
university
entrance diploma is a very central achievement marker in the
German education
system (Wedler et al., 2008). Moreover, one could argue that
SRL as a trait
should also have predictive value for past indicators because
it should not
change that much over time. Both models yield a good fit
(model 1: χ² (42) = 70.73,
p < .01, χ²/df = 1.68, RMSEA = 0.043 [0.024 – 0.059], SRMR
= 0.043, CFI = 0.975,
BIC = 6960.42; model 2: χ² (41) = 63.75, p
< .01, χ²/df =
1.55, RMSEA = 0.038
[0.018 – 0.056], SRMR = 0.041, CFI = 0.981, BIC = 6958.49)
with all significant
factor loadings (p
< .001) and
highly significant correlations with GPA (model 1: r = -0.25, p < .001;
model 2: r = -0.23,
p < .001).
Although the correlation
with GPA is slightly higher for model 1, we depict model 2 in
figure 3 as it
yields better fit indices.
Figure
3. Structural
equation model of trait SRL
and achievement with standardized coefficients. GPA grade point average, SRL
self-regulated learning, COG
cognitive components, CT
critical
thinking, ORG
organization, META
metacognitive components, SEVA self-evaluation,
SREC self-recording,
PLAN planning, MOT
motivational components, VOL
volition, ADOG
academic delay of
gratification, PRO
procrastination, FTP
future time perspective, MB motivational beliefs,
SE self-efficacy, IM
intrinsic motivation, GO
goal
orientation. All factor loadings are significant (p < .001).
4.
Discussion
The present study aimed to
test a comprehensive trait SRL model that integrates volition
besides
cognition, metacognition and motivational beliefs. Moreover, a
new conception
of trait volition for learning was tested empirically. The
results confirm the
hypothesized structure of volition and speak in favour of a
twofold
motivational component for SRL that comprises motivational
beliefs as well as
volition. Furthermore, the trait model of SRL is related to
GPA of university
entrance diploma emphasizing the importance of SRL for
academic achievement.
The first aim of the present
study was to examine a new conception of trait volition for
learning
environments that integrates academic delay of gratification,
future time
perspective and procrastination. The results suggest that
volition within
learning environments comprises the competence of postponing
available
gratification in order to attain important academic goals, and
thus is the
opposite of delaying an intended action. This motivational
regulation is
supported by a personal time frame that is directed to the
future and focuses
on the instrumentality of long-term goals. The present study
therefore answers
some authors’ call for a systematic investigation of the
hypothesized
correlational framework (Bembenutty & Karabenick, 2004)
and is the first to
examine the three constructs concertedly by testing their
underlying structure
empirically. A shortcoming of the conception is the relatively
low factor
loading of academic delay of gratification, which could be
caused by the items’
answer format. As it differs from that of the procrastination
and future time perspective
items, an adaption to the same measurement format could
improve the factor
loading. Although the model is not exhausting, it presents an
integrative and
broad conception that helps to explain and examine trait
volition during
learning.
The second aim of the study
was to test a comprehensive trait model of SRL by extending
the Hong and O’Neil
model (2001) and by incorporating the new conception of trait
volition (e.g.
Duckworth et al., 2014; Wolters & Benzon, 2013). Firstly,
the modelling
results speak in favour of including a cognitive component
that comprises the
usage of organization as well as elaborative learning
strategies. Secondly,
results allow for the conclusion that the metacognitive,
volitional and
motivational belief components should be specified by
considering further
subcomponents. Thirdly, the findings support the assumption of
a comprehensive
motivational component that comprises motivational beliefs
coinciding with
volition. This result clarifies the role of volition as a
critical part of
goal-oriented behaviour: Optimal motivational beliefs are only
valuable if
distractions in the course of action can be handled,
especially when tasks take
several weeks to be completed (Husman et al., 2000). The
motivational component
of the presented model is therefore in agreement with the
differentiation made
in action-control theory: The choice of goals during the
pre-decisional phase
is influenced by self-efficacy, intrinsic value and goal
orientation and
therefore is named choice motivation. The implementation of
planned intentions
in the post-decisional phase however is named executive
motivation and can only
be secured if volition supports motivational beliefs and
therefore regulates
motivation (Zimmerman, 2011). Hence, our model integrates
executive motivation in
addition to choice motivation, which has largely been
neglected within
self-regulated learning research (Garcia et al., 1998).
Nevertheless, future
studies are needed to test sequential aspects of this model.
Longitudinal and
experimental investigations could help to clarify predictive
relations between
motivational beliefs and volitional actions as they should
reflect different
segments of a goal-oriented process.
Although the model with a
twofold motivational component yields a good fit, it is
striking that the
latent second-order motivational variable is represented
strongly by the
first-order volitional factor and only moderately by the
first-order
motivational belief factor. One explanation for this result
could be that the
relationship between motivational beliefs and volition is not
linear, but
rather curvilinear in type. Wolters (2003b) has argued that
highly motivated
students do not need to make use of volitional strategies,
whereas totally
unmotivated students cannot summon the willingness to enact
such strategies.
Volition thus could be regarded as mediating the influence of
motivational
beliefs on the use of SRL strategies (Gaeta, Teruel, &
Orejudo, 2012; Garcia
et al., 1998) or as supportive for student’s motivation in
general (Husman et
al., 2000). Propositions of action-control theory are in line
with the mediator
hypothesis: Whereas motivational beliefs are central
components of the
pre-decisional phase that lead to the choice of a goal,
volition refers to post-decisional
processes that concern the implementation of intentions
(Zimmerman, 2011).
Future research could analyse this hypothesis by conducting
longitudinal
research with a cross-lagged panel design and study the
direction of possible
effects.
An additional shortcoming of
our model is the fact that the motivational belief factor
shows a weak factor
loading for the self-efficacy indicator. This finding is
consistent with the
notion of some authors that self-efficacy is rather a
motivational precursor
than a part of motivation (Usher & Pajares, 2008).
Moreover, only one item used
in the questionnaire referred to self-efficacy for university
context, while four
items asked participants to rate their general self-efficacy
(Jerusalem &
Schwarzer, 1981). As general self-efficacy concerns the
handling of global,
unspecified problems, it is not necessarily related to
academic self-efficacy
(Topkaya, 2010). Thus, it would be more adequate to use items
that measure
academic self-efficacy, or even self-efficacy for
self-regulated learning and
the use of learning strategies (Schunk & Usher, 2011).
The third aim of the present
study was to validate the SRL models by testing their relation
to GPA of
university entrance diploma. While model 1 showed a slightly
higher relation to
GPA, model 2 yielded a better fit. Therefore, we decided to
favour the model
that integrates volition within the motivational component
besides motivational
beliefs as this is in accordance with action-control theory.
Although the
relation with GPA is moderate, it is highly significant and
therefore
emphasizes the importance of SRL for academic achievement. An
explained
variance of about 5% is small but comparable to the results of
similar studies
(e.g. Balkis, Duru, & Bulus, 2013). The inclusion of
interindividual
variables, such as intelligence, personality, or attitudes
could increase the
prediction of GPA. Taken together, the results of the present
study underline
the importance of volition within SRL frameworks and confirm
the construct’s
relevance for academic achievement, especially because
students with high
values on the examined SRL traits show better academic
performance.
4.1
Limitations
Although the data speak in
favour of integrating volition within a broader trait SRL
framework, several
methodological limitations are present that should be
considered when interpreting
the results: The achievement marker used in the present study
is not optimal as
it is retrospective. We chose the criterion of GPA of
university entrance
diploma since it is very central in the German educational
system: It is the
result of national exams that are curricular-based and thus
comparable across
different schools and regions. All university students pass
this exam in the
same class level ensuring a comparable educational level.
Moreover, it is used
for applicant selection at many universities and has a strong
relationship with
later university achievement (Wedler et al., 2008). Therefore,
it is comparable
between students of all subjects of study. Although the
analysis corresponds to
some kind of retrodiction, it is justifiable because we regard
SRL as a stable
trait that should be related to past indicators. Future
studies could analyse
the predictive validity of the integrative SRL model for
current GPA of subject
of study. In order to obtain reliable results, students should
be from the same
field of study because grades of different subjects are not
comparable in the German
college system (Müller-Benedict & Tsarouha, 2011).
Moreover, they should be
in the same phase of their studies in order to obtain
comparable experiences
with university exams. Additionally, future studies should aim
at using
objective achievement markers stemming from performance tests
to validate
models like the one proposed in this study.
Another limitation is the
heterogeneity of the sample used: Participants studied a wide
range of subjects
and were in different phases of their studies. This reduces
the interindividual
comparability concerning study experiences and interest
structures. Nevertheless,
the obtained results using such a heterogeneous sample speak
in favour of SRL
as an important factor for all fields of study. Moreover, the
sample was highly
selective, as college students have the highest school degree
available in
Germany and thus represent the upper ability continuum. Future
studies should
validate SRL models on a sample that is more representative
for the diversity
of our society and the lifelong learners living in it.
Additionally, the
participants were predominantly female. As previous studies
have shown, males
report higher self-efficacy values (Huang, 2013) and show
higher values
concerning the use of elaborative learning strategies
(Bembenutty, 2007).
Nevertheless, females report higher academic delay of
gratification
(Bembenutty, 2007). An exploratory multivariate analysis of
variance using the SRL
and volition subscales as dependent variables and gender as
independent
variable indicated an effect of gender as females in our study
reported
significant higher academic delay of gratification and
significant lower
self-efficacy beliefs than males. Consequently, structural
analyses could be
conducted for both genders separately to investigate if factor
structures
differentiate between the groups.
As mentioned previously, the
different answer formats of the instruments used as well as
the low reliability
for the subscale of organizational learning strategies
represent further
limitations. Future examinations should aim to adjust the
instruments
concerning their structure in order to make them more
comparable and should choose
more reliable items to depict organizational learning
strategies.
4.2
Implications and future directions
The presented findings have
several implications for educational researchers: As the
results speak in favour
of the integration of academic delay of gratification, future
time perspective,
and procrastination to conceptualize volition for learning,
future research
could cross-validate this conception with samples of different
age and
educational groups. In addition, the findings support the
trait SRL model with
an expanded motivational component that integrates
motivational beliefs and
volition. This is why future research should include
volitional factors into the
theoretical research basis along with other motivational
constructs. Moreover,
analyses of the model’s relationship with several variables of
interest, such
as intelligence, personality traits, or attitudes could result
in further
important insights. Latent profile analyses could support the
investigation of
the relation between interpersonal differences and the trait
model of SRL and
could help to indentify types of learners that need different
types of
interventions. Moreover, it would be interesting to examine
the model’s
stability by conducting longitudinal research as stability
measurements would
militate in favour of the trait concept of SRL. Questionnaire
methods are
indispensible for measuring traits because participants have
to aggregate their
behaviour concerning several situations, which is in
accordance with the
situation-independent trait perspective. Nevertheless, future
research could
complement self-report scales with qualitative instruments,
such as interviews
or thinking aloud protocols (Veenman, 2011), representing a
multimethod
approach. Subsequently, with regard to the analysis of trait
SRL, future
studies could transfer the model to state level and examine
its structure using
process measures. As trait and state SRL are highly
interrelated (Hong, 1998),
it would be interesting to systematically analyse whether the
components of our
model are present in all phases of SRL using process models as
a theoretical
basis (e.g. Zimmerman, 2000). Moreover, state analyses could
focus the question
which strategies are used to ensure volitional control.
Altogether, it seems
appropriate to assume that cognitive and metacognitive
variables as well as
motivational beliefs and volition are important for planning,
performing, and
reflecting upon one’s learning. Nevertheless, more research is
needed to derive
practical implications based on these theoretical findings.
Longitudinal
studies that investigate the stability of the constructs,
their reciprocal
relations as well as their development and interconnection in
earlier stages of
life could be helpful.
Keypoints
Academic delay of
gratification, procrastination and
future time perspective can be integrated in order to depict
volition for SRL.
An SRL trait model
that comprises cognitive,
metacognitive and motivational components yields a good fit.
Volition for
learning can be integrated within that model by extending
the motivational
component and adding volition above and beyond motivational
beliefs.
The proposed SRL
trait model is related to GPA which
is a first hint of its validity.
Acknowledgments
The
authors wish to thank the Doctoral
Training Programme of Saarland University (GradUS) for funding
the present
research.
References
Akinsola,
M.
K., Tella, A., & Tella, A. (2007). Correlates of academic
procrastination and mathematics achievement of university
undergraduate
students. Eurasia Journal
of Mathematics,
Science & Technology Education, 3(4), 363-370.
Azevedo, R.
(2014). Issues in dealing with sequential and temporal
characteristics of
self-and socially-regulated learning. Metacognition and
Learning, 9(2),
217-228. doi:10.1007/s11409-014-9123-1
Balkis, M., Duru,
E., & Bulus, M. (2013). Analysis of the relation between
academic
procrastination, academic rational/irrational beliefs, time
preferences to
study for exams, and academic achievement: a structural model. European
Journal
of Psychology of Education, 28(3), 825-839. doi:10.1007/s10212-012-0142-5
Bandura, A.
(1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura,
A. (1997). Self-efficacy:
The exercise of
control. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Bembenutty,
H. (2007). Self-Regulation of Learning and Academic Delay of
Gratification:
Gender and Ethnic Differences among College Students. Journal
Of Advanced
Academics, 18(4), 586-616. doi:10.4219/jaa-2007-553
Bembenutty,
H. (2008). Academic delay of gratification and expectancy–value.
Personality
and Individual Differences, 44(1), 193-202. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.07.025
Bembenutty,
H. (2009). Teaching effectiveness, course evaluation, and
academic performance:
The role of academic delay of gratification. Journal of
Advanced Academics,
20(2), 326-355. doi:10.1177/1932202X0902000206
Bembenutty, H.,
& Karabenick, S. A. (1998). Academic delay of gratification.
Learning and
Individual Differences, 10(4), 329-346. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80126-5
Bembenutty, H.,
& Karabenick, S. A. (2004). Inherent association between
academic delay of
gratification, future time perspective, and self-regulated
learning. Educational
Psychology Review, 16(1), 35-57. doi:10.1023/B:EDPR.0000012344.34008.5c
Boekaerts,
M. (1999). Self-regulated learning: Where we are today. International
Journal
of Educational Research, 31(6), 445-457. doi:10.1016/S0883-0355(99)00014-2
Boekaerts,
M., & Corno, L. (2005). Self‐regulation in the classroom: A
perspective on
assessment and intervention. Applied Psychology, 54(2),
199-231.
doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00205.x
Bronson, M.
B. (2000). Self-regulation
in early
childhood. Nature and Nurture. New York: The Guilford
Press.
Burnham, K.
P. & Anderson, D. R. (2004). Multimodel Inference:
Understanding AIC and BIC
in Model Selection. Sociological
Methods
& Research, 33(2), 261-304. doi:10.1177/0049124104268644
Carver,
C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2000).On the structure of
behavioral
self-regulation. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M.
Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook
of self-regulation (pp 42-80).
San Diego: Academic Press.
Corno, L.
(1993). The best-laid plans: modern conceptions of volition and
educational
research. Educational researcher, 22(2), 14-22.
doi:10.3102/0013189X022002014
Corno, L. (1994). Student
volition and education:
Outcomes, influences, and practices. In D. H. Schunk & B.
J. Zimmerman
(Eds.), Self-regulation
of learning and
performance: Issues and educational applications (pp.
229-251). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Corno, L. (2001).
Volitional aspects of self-regulated
learning. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement.
Theoretical
perspectives (pp. 191-226). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Corno, L., & Kanfer, R.
(1993). The role of volition in learning and performance. In
L. Darling-Hammond
(Ed.), Review of
Research in Education
(Vol. 21, pp. 301-341). Itasca, IL: F.E. Peacock Publishers.
de Bilde,
J., Vansteenkiste, M., & Lens, W. (2011). Understanding the
association
between future time perspective and self-regulated learning
through the lens of
self-determination theory. Learning and Instruction, 21(3),
332-344.
doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.03.002
Dewitte, S.,
& Lens, W. (2000). Procrastinators lack a broad action
perspective. European
Journal of Personality, 14(2), 121-140. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(200003/04)14:2<121::AID-PER368>3.0.CO;2-#
DiBenedetto, M.
K., & Bembenutty, H. (2013). Within the pipeline:
Self-regulated learning,
self-efficacy, and socialization among college students in
science courses. Learning
and Individual Differences, 23, 218-224. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.09.015
Dietz, F.,
Hofer, M., & Fries, S. (2007). Individual values, learning
routines and
academic procrastination. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 77(4),
893-906. doi:10.1348/000709906X169076
Dignath, C.,
Buettner, G., & Langfeldt, H. P. (2008). How can primary
school students
learn self-regulated learning strategies most effectively?: A
meta-analysis on
self-regulation training programmes. Educational Research
Review, 3(2),
101-129. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2008.02.003
Duckworth,
A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Self-control
in school-age
children. Educational Psychologist, 49(3),
199-217. doi:10.1080/00461520.2014.926225
Duckworth,
A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2006). Self-discipline gives girls
the edge:
Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores.
Journal of
Educational Psychology, 98(1), 198. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198
Efklides, A.
(2011). Interactions of metacognition with motivation and affect
in
self-regulated learning: The MASRL model. Educational
Psychologist, 46(1),
6-25. doi:10.1080/00461520.2011.538645
Gaeta, M.
L., Teruel M. P., & Orejudo, S. (2012). Motivational,
volitional and
metacognitive aspects of self regulated learning. Electronic
Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 10(1),
73-94.
Garcia, T.,
McCann, E. J., Turner, J. E., & Roska, L. (1998). Modeling
the mediating
role of volition in the learning process. Contemporary
Educational
Psychology, 23(4), 392-418. doi:10.1006/ceps.1998.0982
Geiser, C.
(2011). Datenanalyse mit
MPlus [Data analysis using MPlus]. Wiesbaden: VS Verlag.
Gollwitzer, P. M.
(1996). The volitional benefits of planning. In P. M.
Gollwitzer, & J. A. Barge (Eds.), The
psychology of
action (pp. 287-312). New York: Guilford Press.
Heckhausen, H., & Kuhl,
J. (1985). From
wishes to action: The dead ends and short cuts on the long way
to action. In M.
Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.), Goal-directed
behavior: The concept of action in psychology (pp.
134-160). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Hertzog, C.,
& Nesselroade, J. R. (2003). Assessing psychological change
in adulthood:
an overview of methodological issues. Psychology and Aging,
18(4),
639. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.639
Hong, E.
(1995). A structural comparison between state and trait self‐regulation
models. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
9(4), 333-349. doi:10.1002/acp.2350090406
Hong, E.
(1998). Differential stability of state and trait
self-regulation in academic
performance. The Journal of Educational Research, 91(3),
148-159.
doi:10.1080/00220679809597536
Hong, E.,
& O'Neil Jr, H. F. (2001). Construct validation of a trait
self-regulation
model. International Journal of Psychology, 36(3),
186-194. doi:10.1080/00207590042000146
Horstmanshof,
L., & Zimitat, C. (2007). Future time orientation predicts
academic
engagement among first‐year
university students. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 77(3),
703-718. doi:10.1348/000709906X160778
Howell, A.
J., & Watson, D. C. (2007). Procrastination: Associations
with achievement
goal orientation and learning strategies. Personality and
Individual
Differences, 43(1), 167-178. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.017
Huang, C.
(2013). Gender differences in academic self-efficacy: A
meta-analysis. European
Journal Of Psychology Of Education, 28(1), 1-35.
doi:10.1007/s10212-011-0097-y
Husman, J.,
McCann, E., & Crowson, H. M. (2000). Volitional strategies
and future time
perspective: embracing the complexity of dynamic interactions. International
Journal
of Educational Research, 33(7), 777-799. doi:10.1016/S0883-0355(00)00050-1
Jerusalem,
M., & Schwarzer, R. (1981). Selbstwirksamkeit
[Self-Efficacy]. In R. Schwarzer
(Eds.), Skalen zur
Befindlichkeit und
Persönlichkeit [Scales to measure mood and personality]
(pp. 15-28). Berlin: Freie Universität
Berlin.
Keller, J.
M. (2008). An integrative theory of motivation, volition, and
performance. Technology,
Instruction, Cognition, and Learning, 6(2),
79-104.
Kitsantas,
A. (2002). Test preparation and performance: A self-regulatory
analysis. The
Journal of Experimental Education, 70(2), 101-113.
doi:10.1080/00220970209599501
Kitsantas, A.,
Winsler, A., & Huie, F. (2008). Self-regulation and ability
predictors of academic
success during college: A predictive validity study. Journal
of Advanced
Academics, 20(1), 42-68. doi:10.4219/jaa-2008-867
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation
Modeling (2nd ed.).
New York: Guilford.
Kuhl, J. (1984). Volitional
aspects of
achievement motivation and learned helplessness: Toward a
comprehensive theory
of action control. In B. A. Maher & W. B. Maher (Eds.), Progress in Experimental
Personality
Research (pp. 101-171). Orlando: Academic Press.
Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional
mediators of
cognition-behaviour consistency. Self-regulatory processes and
action vs. state
orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behavior (pp.
101-128). New York:
Springer.
Kuhl, J. (2000). The
Volitional Basis of
Personality Systems Interaction Theory: Applications in
Learning and Treatment
Contexts. International Journal Of Educational Research,
33(7-8),
665-703. doi:10.1016/S0883-0355(00)00045-8
Little, R. J. A., &
Rubin, D. B. (2002). Statistical
analysis with missing data.
Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.
Luszczynska, A., &
Sutton, S. (2006).
Physical activity after cardiac rehabilitation: Evidence that
different types
of self-efficacy are important in maintainers and relapsers. Rehabilitation
Psychology, 51(4), 314-321. doi:10.1037/0090-5550.51.4.314
Matthews, G., Schwean, V.
L., Campbell, S.
E., Saklofske, D. H., & Mohamed, A. R. (2000).
Personality,
self-regulation, and adaptation: A cognitive-social framework.
In M. Boekaerts,
P. R. Pintrich, M. Zeidner, M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, M.
Zeidner (Eds.) , Handbook
of self-regulation (pp. 171-207). San Diego, CA, US:
Academic Press.
Miller, R.
B., & Brickman, S. J. (2004). A model of future-oriented
motivation and
self-regulation. Educational Psychology Review, 16(1),
9-33. doi:10.1023/B:EDPR.0000012343.96370.39
Müller-Benedict,
V., & Tsarouha, E. (2011). Können Examensnoten verglichen
werden? Eine Analyse von Einflüssen des sozialen Kontextes auf
Hochschulprüfungen [Are grades in exams comparable to each
other? The impact of social context on grading in higher
education]. Zeitschrift für Soziologie, 388-409.
Muthén, L. K., &
Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus:
Statistical
analysis with latent variables
(version
7) [Computer software]. Los Angeles: Authors.
Nandagopal, K.,
& Ericsson, K. A. (2012). An expert performance approach
to the study of individual differences in self-regulated
learning activities in
upper-level college students. Learning
and Individual Differences, 22, 597–609. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.018
Park, S. W.,
& Sperling, R. A. (2012). Academic procrastinators and their
self-regulation. Psychology, 3(01), 12. doi:10.4236/psych.2012.31003
Peetsma, T.,
Schuitema, J., & Van der Veen, I. (2012). A longitudinal
study on time perspectives: Relations
with academic delay of gratification and learning environment. Japanese
Psychological
Research, 54(3), 241-252. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00526.x
Pintrich, P. R. (1999).
Taking control of
research on volitional control: Challenges for future theory
and research. Learning
and Individual Differences, 11(3),
335-354. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80007-7
Pintrich,
P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated
learning. In M.
Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 451-502). San
Diego: Academic
Press.
Pintrich,
P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J.
(1991). A manual for the
use of the Motivated
Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Michigan:
University of
Michigan.
Rakes, G. C.,
& Dunn, K. E. (2010). The impact of online graduate
students’ motivation and
self-regulation on academic procrastination. Journal of
Interactive Online
Learning, 9(1), 78-93.
Schermelleh-Engel,
K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit
of structural equation models:
Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures.
Methods of
psychological research online, 8(2), 23-74.
Schmitz, B.,
& Wiese, B. S. (2006). New perspectives for the evaluation
of training
sessions in self-regulated learning: Time-series analyses of
diary data. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 31(1), 64-96. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.02.002
Schouwenburg, H. C. (2004).
Academic
procrastination: Theoretical notions, measurement, and
research. In H. C.
Schouwenburg, C. H. Lay, T. A. Pychyl, & J. R. Ferrari
(Eds.), Counseling the
procrastinator in academic
settings (pp. 3–17). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Schunk, D.,
& Usher, E. (2011). Assessing self-efficacy for
self-regulated learning. In
B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of
self-regulation of learning
and performance (p. 282-297). New
York, NY: Routledge.
Simons, J.,
Dewitte, S., & Lens, W. (2004). The Role of Different Types
of
Instrumentality in Motivation, Study Strategies, and
Performance: Know Why You
Learn, So You'll know What You Learn!. British Journal Of
Educational
Psychology, 74(3), 343-360. doi:10.1348/0007099041552314
Sirois, F.
M. (2014). Out of sight, out of time? A meta‐analytic
investigation of procrastination and time
perspective. European Journal of Personality, 28(5),
511-520. doi:10.1002/per.1947
Steel, P.
(2007). The nature of procrastination: a meta-analytic and
theoretical review
of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological
Bulletin, 133(1),
65-94. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65
Topkaya, E.
Z. (2010). Pre-Service English Language Teachers' Perceptions of
Computer
Self-Efficacy and General Self-Efficacy. Turkish Online
Journal of
Educational Technology, 9(1), 143-156.
Tuckman, B.
W. (1991). The development and concurrent validity of the
procrastination
scale. Educational and psychological measurement, 51(2),
473-480.
doi:10.1177/0013164491512022
Usher, E.
L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Self-efficacy for self-regulated
learning: A
validation study. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
68(3),
443-463. doi:10.1177/0013164407308475
Veenman, M.
V. (2011). Alternative assessment of strategy use with
self-report instruments:
a discussion. Metacognition and Learning, 6(2),
205-211. doi:10.1007/s11409-011-9080-x
Wild, K., &
Schiefele, U. (1994). Lernstrategien im
Studium: Ergebnisse zur Faktorenstruktur und Reliabilität eines
neuen
Fragebogens [Learning strategies in college: Results concerning
the factorial
structure and reliability of a new questionnaire]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische
Psychologie, 15(4),
185-200.
Winne, P. H.,
& Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-regulated
learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich & M. Zeidner
(Eds.), Handbook of
self-regulation (pp.
532-568). San Diego: Academic Press.
Wolters, C.
A. (1999). The relation between high school students'
motivational regulation
and their use of learning strategies, effort, and classroom
performance. Learning
and Individual Differences, 11(3), 281-299. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80004-1
Wolters, C.
A. (2003a). Understanding procrastination from a self-regulated
learning
perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1),
179. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.179
Wolters, C.
A. (2003b). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating an
underemphasized aspect of
self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(4),
189-205.
doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3804_1
Wolters, C. A.,
& Benzon, M. B. (2013). Assessing and predicting college
students’ use of
strategies for the self-regulation of motivation. The
Journal of
Experimental Education, 81(2), 199-221. doi:10.1080/00220973.2012.699901
Wolters, C.
A., & Hussain, M. (2014). Investigating grit and its
relations with college
students’ self-regulated learning and academic achievement. Metacognition
And
Learning, 1-19. doi:10.1007/s11409-014-9128-9
Zimbardo, P.
G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A
valid, reliable
individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,
77(6), 1271-1288. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271
Zimmerman,
B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive
perspective. In M.
Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13 – 41). San
Diego: Academic
Press.
Zimmerman,
B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation and motivation:
Historical
background, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational
Research Journal, 45(1),
166–183. doi:10.3102/0002831207312909
Zimmerman,
B. J. (2011). Motivational sources and outcomes of
self-regulated learning and
performance. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
of learning and
performance (pp. 49-64). New York, NY: Routledge.
Zimmerman, B. J.,
& Kitsantas, A. (2014). Comparing students’ self-discipline
and
self-regulation measures and their prediction of academic
achievement. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 39(2), 145-155. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.03.004
Zimmerman, B. J.,
& Schunk, D. H. (2001). Self-regulated learning and
academic achievement:
Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ, US:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
[1]
Corresponding
author: Laura Dörrenbächer, Department of Educational
Sciences, Saarland
University, Campus Building A 4 2, 66123 Saarbrücken,
Germany. Phone: +49(0)681
/ 30258337, Fax: +49(0)681/ 30258341, Email: laura.doerrenbaecher@uni-saarland.de
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.14786/flr.v3i4.179
[2]
The term motivation will be used to
refer to motivational components in general, while
motivation beliefs refer to
self-beliefs concerning self-efficacy, goal orientation or
intrinsic task value
and are distinguished from volition as a second motivational
component.