Effects of a
Short Strategy Training on Metacognitive Monitoring across
the Life-span
Nicole von der Linden[1],
Elisabeth Löffler, Wolfgang Schneider
University of Würzburg, Germany
Article received 29 July /
revised 20 November
/ accepted 21 November / available online 18
January
Abstract
The present study was conducted to explore the
potential positive influence of a short strategy training on
metacognitive monitoring competencies covering a life-span
approach. Participants of four age groups (3rd-grade
children, adolescents, younger and older adults) concluded a
paired-associate learning task. Additionally, they gave
delayed Judgments-of-Learning (JOLs), that is, they rated
their certainty that they would later be able to recall
specific details correctly, and Confidence Judgements (CJs),
that is, they rated their certainty that the provided
answers in the recall test were correct. Half of the
participants underwent a short strategy training in order to
enhance their recollection of contextual details thus
providing a diagnostic basis for forming metacognitive
judgements. Results revealed significant gains in memory
performance after completing the strategy training.
Moreover, a positive effect of the strategy training on JOLs
and CJs differentiation and accuracy could be detected.
Effects were most pronounced for children and older adults.
Participants who had completed the strategy training also
reported a decrease of familiarity-based metacognitive
judgments and were able to identify memories for which no
reliable cues existed more easily than participants in the
control condition. Accordingly, improvements in monitoring
performance seemed to be due to a shift in underlying cues.
In sum, this study integrates traditional aims from the
relatively separately existing lines of metacognitive
research in the developmental and cognitive literature and
adds to understanding and improving monitoring judgments in
a lifetime sample.
Keywords: metamemory, judgments-of-learning,
confidence judgments, monitoring, life-span, strategy
instruction
[1] Corresponding author: Nicole von der
Linden, Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg,
Röntgenring 10, 97070 Würzburg, Germany. Phone: +49(0)931 / 31
89067, Fax: +49(0)931 / 31 2763, Email:
linden@psychologie.uni-wuerzburg.de
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.14786/flr.v3i4.196
1. Introduction
Accurate
metacognitive monitoring plays an important role in many
everyday situations as well as in learning contexts
(Schneider, 2010; Son & Metcalfe, 2000). In daily
routines, metacognitive monitoring is for instance relevant
when one has to decide about whether one has memorized the
departure time of one’s train, or whether one has taken
appropriate notes of a lecture. Moreover, subjective
monitoring judgments influence learning behaviors,
especially the selection of to-be-studied items and the
allocation of study time (see Son & Metcalfe, 2000, for
a review). Structured learning situations are not only
important to children and young adults but also for
life-long learning which has gained importance in recent
years. Yet, life-span perspective is still rare in the
metacognitive monitoring literature.
Metacognitive
research
has traditionally been conducted within two main but
separate lines of research: a developmental perspective
(Flavell, 1999) which focuses on the changes of
metacognitive abilities during life and a cognitive
tradition (for an overview cf. Dunlosky & Metcalfe,
2009; Koriat & Levy-Sardot, 1999) which tries to explore
mechanisms underlying metacognitive monitoring processes and
its consequences for regulation of learning typically in an
adult sample only. In the following, we present a study
which is one of few existing attempts to combine both
perspectives concerning metacognitive monitoring processes.
Firstly, the present study aimed at exploring developmental
trajectories of monitoring abilities in a life-span sample
from early school age to older adulthood. A second purpose
was to improve participants’ monitoring competencies by
reinforcing highly diagnostic cues in paired-associate
learning situations (McCabe & Soderstrom, 2011;
Robinson, Hertzog, & Dunlosky, 2006) and to investigate
the role of familiarity and recollection-based cues for
monitoring processes.
In
learning situations two aspects of monitoring are of special
interest: Judgments of Learning (JOL) and Confidence
Judgments (CJ). According to Nelson and Narens’ (1990)
seminal model of procedural metamemory, JOLs provide
subjective information about the degree to which encoded
information has been mastered and can be potentially
recalled during a future memory test (Nelson & Narens,
1990). Findings from studies using different age groups
suggest that even young children can effectively monitor
their learning progress under certain circumstances. On the
one hand, the results indicate that immediate JOLs are
typically inaccurate and also represent overestimations of
one’s actual performance. Remarkably, this is true not only
for children of different ages but also for adults.
Immediately after studying new information, judgments about
its future recall seem severely biased by the false belief
that information currently in short-term memory can be
easily recalled some minutes later. Obviously, this bias
operates similarly in participants of different ages. On the
other hand, however, even young children can make rather
accurate assessments of the subsequent recallability of
items when this judgment is somewhat delayed, that is, when
it takes place a minute or two after studying the item. In
other words, even young children seem to have a good feeling
for which items will be recallable and which will not when
long-term memory information has to be accessed for the JOL
(Schneider, 2015).
Confidence
judgments (CJs) concern retrieval monitoring and are
typically made after a response is given to indicate how
sure participants are about the correctness of an answer.
CJs are thought to reflect a substantive sense of certainty
that arises from the strength of the memory that is being
retrieved, and this sense of certainty has been interpreted
as an indicator of memory accuracy (Ghetti, Lyons, Lazzarin,
& Cornoldi, 2008; Roebers, 2002).
Metacognitive
monitoring
judgments are commonly believed to be based on multiple
cues. This accessibility view has been proposed for
immediate and delayed JOLs (Koriat, 1997; Metcalfe &
Finn, 2008; Toth, Daniels, & Solinger, 2011) as well as
for CJs (Kelley & Jacoby, 1996; Kelley & Sahakyan,
2003). The accuracy of those judgments depends on whether
accessible cues are diagnostic of memory performance or not
(Dunlosky & Metcalfe, 2009). Cues are highly diagnostic
if they influence metacognitive judgments and recall
performance in a similar way. Thus far multiple sources
involved in the construction of monitoring judgments have
been postulated. For example, research investigating
immediate JOLs emphasizes encoding fluency as a major base,
which in turn depends on different factors such as the
concreteness and the frequency of the items (Begg, Duft,
Lalonde, Melnick, & Sanvito, 1989) as well as
familiarity of items (Nelson & Narens, 1990). Delayed
JOLs have been linked to retrieval fluency (Benjamin &
Bjork, 1996) as well as success and ease of item retrieval
(Nelson, Narens, & Dunlosky, 2004). Similar to JOLs, for
CJs a number of different cues have been discussed to
influence accuracy, among them perceived ease (Zakay &
Tuvia, 1998) and vividness of retrieval (Robinson, Johnson,
& Robertson, 2000).
Different
attempts have been made to categorize these various types of
cues (Koriat, 1997; Kelley & Jacoby, 1996). In recent
years the literature has begun to discuss the distinction
between familiarity-based and recollection-based cues for
different metacognitive judgments (Daniels, Toth, &
Hertzog, 2009; McCabe & Soderstrom, 2011; Metcalfe &
Finn, 2008; Toth et al., 2011). Recollection is typically
defined as the consciously controlled intentional use of
memory that allows for the retrieval of qualitative details
of a past event. This process is frequently associated with
the subjective experience of vivid remembering. Familiarity,
by contrast, usually refers to experiences of prior events
that may arise from activated semantic representations. The
relative contribution of recollection and familiarity may
differ from task to task. Memory tasks that require
participants to recall or recognize details about the target
item rely heavily on recollection. So far, the literature
lacks a systematic examination of the role of recollection
and familiarity cues across different monitoring indicators
and across the life-span.
Evidence
suggests that both immediate (Daniels et al., 2009; Toth et
al., 2011) and delayed JOLs (Metcalfe & Finn, 2008) are
influenced by recollection and familiarity processes. Yet,
delayed JOLs are mainly based on cues related to
recollection such as target retrievability (for an overview
see Metcalfe & Finn, 2008), whereas familiarity
processes, such as processing fluency, have been identified
as primary cues for immediate JOLs in younger adults
(Matvey, Dunlosky, & Guttentag, 2001; Rhodes &
Castel, 2008). To our knowledge, the role of familiarity and
recollection processes underlying JOLs in children has not
been examined to date. Concerning older adults, first
evidence suggests that they have more problems with
monitoring recollection processes than younger adults
(Daniels et al., 2009). Several findings support the idea
that recollection processes increase the accuracy of
monitoring processes. First, as noted above, delayed JOLs
have been shown to be more accurate than immediate JOLs for
children, younger, and older adults (Connor, Dunlosky, &
Hertzog, 1997; Koriat & Shitzer-Reichert, 2002; Nelson
& Dunlosky, 1991; Schneider,Visé, Lockl, & Nelson,
2000). This can be explained by the fact that for delayed
JOLs participants actively assess long-term memory
(recollection processes), which is more predictive of recall
than short-term memory used for immediate JOLs.
Additionally, with delayed JOLs participants seem to rely
more on idiosyncratic cues of encoding and remembering than
with immediate JOLs (Koriat, 1997). Idiosyncratic cues refer
to personal, item-specific details, for instance, images or
associations. Providing a rich basis of idiosyncratic cues
(e.g. through a strategy training) should facilitate the
identification of recollection-based memories. Further
evidence for the importance of recollection processes for
accurate monitoring in younger adults is provided by the
following fact: Focusing participants’ attention to cues
connected with target retrievability enhanced JOL accuracy
compared to immediate JOLs (McCabe & Soderstrom, 2011).
Furthermore, for recollection-based memories higher (Daniels
et al., 2009; Toth et al., 2011) and more accurate immediate
JOLs (Toth et al., 2011) could be found than for
familiarity-based memories. In sum, more research on the
role of familiarity and recollection processes for JOLs is
needed, not only for children but also in terms of
comparisons of broader age ranges. Yet, existing evidence
points to the fact that the retrieval of contextual
information seems to provide a reliable cue for later memory
performance in different age groups.
Similarly,
CJs seem to be based on recollection (analytic) or
familiarity (non-analytic) components (Kelley & Jacoby,
1996; Kelley & Sahakyan, 2003). Recollection processes
have been identified as playing a major role for CJ accuracy
(Kelley & Sahakyan, 2003) and accuracy losses with
increasing old age have been linked to recollection
impairments (Kelley & Sahakyan, 2003; Wong, Cramer,
& Gallo, 2012). For children the role of recollection
and familiarity processes underlying CJs has not been
addressed yet. Remember-know-judgments which are positively
correlated with CJs (Holmes & Weaver, 2010) also
emphasize the role of recollection and familiarity processes
for metacognitive judgments. Remember-know-judgments
distinguish whether a memory content is associated with
specific contextual information (recollection) or is
familiarity-based, with participants unable to retrieve the
personal encounter with a memory detail.
Although
there is consensus that participants base their monitoring
judgments on various cues, they do not always take into
consideration the factors which are most predictive of
memory performance (Koriat, 1997; Touron, Hertzog, &
Speagle, 2010). As discussed above, recollection-based cues
are considered to be important and also highly diagnostic
for both JOLs and CJs. Therefore, a training program that
aims at strengthening the accessibility of those cues should
enhance monitoring accuracy (McCabe & Soderstrom, 2011).
Yet, to our knowledge no systematic training in this area
has been carried out. A training should be beneficial for
all age groups but especially for older adults, among whom
deficits in monitoring processes have been linked to
problems with recollection processes for JOLs (Toth et al.,
2011), CJs (Shing, Werkle-Bergner, Li, & Lindenberger,
2009; Wong et al., 2012) and Feeling-of-knowing judgments
(Souchay, Bacon, & Danion, 2006). Children should also
particularly profit from such a training procedure as
developmental progression in monitoring skills seems to be
influenced by improved retrieval processes for both JOLs
(Koriat & Shitzer-Reichert, 2002) and CJs (Roderer &
Roebers, 2011).
The
study presented here was designed to fill this gap in the
literature and to explore the effect of recollection and
familiarity based cues on metacognitive monitoring. Although
the role of recollection and familiarity-based processes for
metacognitive processes has received more attention in
recent years, available studies have focused on one type of
metacognitive judgment only and involve one or at most two
age groups (Daniels et al., 2009; Souchay et al., 2006).
Especially empirical studies with children are scarce.
Therefore the design included a life-span perspective to
account for the life-long significance of learning.
Moreover, two different types of metacognitive judgments
(JOLs and CJs) were included in the present study in order
to allow for direct comparison within one sample. Multiple
but partly different cues seem to underlie delayed JOLs and
CJs (Koriat, 1997, 2012) for they occur in different stages
of the learning process. Compared to JOLs, strengthening
recollection processes may have a somewhat greater effect on
CJs as the longer interval to the learning stage might
otherwise foster the reliance on familiarity, especially in
older adults (Shing et al., 2009). Consequently, it is of
interest to compare different monitoring indicators yet such
studies are very rare in the literature (Leonesio &
Nelson, 1990).
Specifically,
participants
of four age groups (early school age to later adulthood)
were asked to complete a paired-associate learning task and
to give delayed JOLs (which are more accurate than immediate
JOLs across all of the different age groups; see above) and
CJs. A paired-associate task was chosen in order to ensure
comparability with related studies and because for this
stimulus material ample evidence exists for the effectivity
of strategy trainings across all included age groups (see
below). Half of the participants underwent a strategy
training in order to enhance their recollection of
contextual details during JOL, CJ and test collection, thus
providing a diagnostic basis for forming metacognitive
judgments. To ensure that the training was transferable to
rehearsing processes in everyday life and for different age
groups a short instruction in mental imagery was chosen. In
paired-associate learning, mental imagery has proven to be
the most efficient way of processing (Richardson, 1998), and
it effectively improves recall performance in different age
groups from first grade on to older age (Richardson, 1998;
Verhaeghen, Marcoen, & Goossens, 1992; Willoughby,
Porter, Belsito, & Yearsley, 1999).
Even
more relevant to our study, some recent studies provide
first evidence for the fact that strategy use successfully
improves monitoring processes for both JOLs and CJs although
this research does not specifically explore the cues
underlying monitoring judgments and hardly ever an explicit
strategy training was done. Hertzog, Sinclair, and Dunlosky
(2010) have shown that spontaneous strategy use (e.g. mental
imagery), which was not induced but only accessed after JOL
collection, substantially influenced JOLs and JOL
resolution, that is, the accuracy with which a person can
monitor the relative recallability of different items, in
adults aged 18 to 81. Robinson et al. (2006) instructed but
not trained younger and older adults to use a mental imagery
strategy when memorizing pairs of items. They found that the
size of the JOLs and recall performance were positively
correlated with strategy use in both age groups and mental
imagery was identified as a diagnostic cue for JOLs. To our
knowledge no comparable studies exist for children. Thus in
JOLs, so far no attempts have been made to directly train
subjects of a broad age range to apply an imagery strategy.
Concerning
CJs, Nietfeld and Schraw (2002) trained college students to
use various strategies for probability tests. As a result,
subjects benefitted from the instructions both in terms of
performance and monitoring accuracy (CJs). Besides Shing et
al. (2009) showed that participants from 10 to 75 years of
age benefitted from strategy training in terms of their CJs
by enlarging the difference in CJs provided after hits
compared to false alarms.
In
accordance with the literature we expected a positive effect
of strategy training on both recall processes and
metacognitive processes (JOLs and CJs) in all age groups. As
our study is the first to include a life-span approach to
investigate the influence of recollection processes on
metacognitive monitoring, developmental effects were of
special interest. We proposed that children and older adults
would benefit most from the strategy training: production
deficits concerning strategy use are most pronounced in
children and older adults (Naveh-Benjamin, Brav, & Levy,
2007; Pressley & Levin, 1977), and recall performance
increases during childhood declines in older adulthood
(Weinert & Schneider, 1996). Although generally little
developmental progression is found for JOLs, recent evidence
suggests that under certain circumstances deficits in
recollection processes may play a role in lower JOL accuracy
in older adults (Daniels et al., 2009; Toth et al., 2011).
As for CJs, their accuracy has been shown to improve over
the primary school years (Roebers, von der Linden, Howie,
& Schneider, 2007), and they seem to be influenced by
retrieval processes (Roderer & Roebers, 2010). Deficits
in older adults’ CJs have also been linked to deteriorated
recollection processes (Kelley & Sahakyan, 2003).
Additionally, we aimed to compare the effects of our
training on different monitoring indicators as the
importance of recollection processes might vary in different
stages of the learning process.
Since
we proposed that a strategy training should be effective
mainly due to enhanced accessibility of recollection-based
cues, we additionally asked participants to classify the
basis of their recall as Recollection, Familiarity or No
Memory (RFN-judgments). These classifications were
successfully introduced for JOLs by Daniels et al. (2009)
and Toth et al. (2011). We extended the use of RFN-judgments
to CJs.
2. Method
2.1 Sample
A
total of 160 (85 male, 75 female) participants of four age
groups (40 children in 3rd grade, 40 adolescents in 7th and
8th grade, 40 younger adults between 19 and 26 years of age
and 40 older adults between 60 and 75 years) took part in
our study. This sample size surpasses the required number of
N = 132
participants as determined by an a-priori power-analysis
which was conducted with the premise to detect medium-sized
effects according to Cohen (d = .25). They were recruited via
contacting their schools directly and via newspaper and
internet advertisements. Children and adolescents received
small gifts, whereas the other participants got 10-Euro
vouchers or were paid in cash. Subjects’ mean ages were 8.38
(SD = 0.49) for
the children, 12.73 (SD
= 0.72) for the adolescents, 22.75 (SD = 2.02) for the
younger adults and 68.40 (SD = 4.08) for the older adults.
2.2 Materials
The
learning items consisted of pairs of concrete German nouns
from different semantic categories (e.g. zoo animals,
furniture, clothing etc.). To vary the difficulty, one half
of the pairs represented two words from the same category
and the other half of the pairs comprised words from two
different categories. The item list for children and older
adults consisted of 45 word pairs in the study phase and 60
word pairs in the recognition phase (the latter including 30
pairs identical to the study phase, 15 newly matched pairs
and 15 completely new pairs). Item pairs for adolescents
included 54 word pairs in the study phase and 72 items in
the recognition phase. The numbers for younger adults were
60 and 80 item pairs respectively. Four practice pairs not
included in the analysis preceded each single phase of the
experiment.
The
appearance of word pairs as identical or recombined was
counterbalanced among the subjects. The order of
presentation was randomized as well.
2.3 Procedure
The
consent of the parents and of the school was obtained for
the children and adolescents before the beginning of the
study. Participants were tested individually in quiet rooms
in the school or in the laboratory.
Half
of the subjects of each age group were randomly assigned to
the strategy-instruction condition (experimental group).
They received instructions on visual imagery. The test
administrator first explained the advantages of memorizing
word pairs as one interconnected image emphasizing the
importance of integration of the two images. The explanation
was facilitated by two drawings, one of a frog carrying a
banana and one of a candle burning a letter. Then the
subjects in the experimental condition had to practice this
visual imagery strategy by means of ten word pairs which
were different from those used in the experiment. They were
given feedback on the quality of their imagery and were
asked to imagine another combined image if necessary. The
instruction was standardized for all participants with the
restriction that the wording in children’s was slightly
simplified. Participants of all age groups reported that
they easily understood the strategy. The participants in the
experimental group were instructed to use the visual imagery
strategy while memorizing the items. In the control group no
strategy instruction was given.
The
word pairs were presented on a computer screen with
presentation rates of 8 seconds per item pair for children
and older adults, 6 seconds for adolescents, and 2.5 seconds
for younger adults. The presentation rates were adapted in
order to control for baseline difficulty between the age
groups. Subjects were instructed to concentrate on the pairs
because they later would have to recognize them and to
indicate whether the word pair had appeared in the study
phase or not.
In the
JOL phase, each left noun of the item pair (stimulus) was
presented on the screen in the same order as in the learning
phase. To avoid relearning only the stimulus was shown.
Subjects were asked to indicate the likelihood of
recognizing the word pair in about 30 minutes. JOLs were
rated on a thermometer scale from 0 (very unsure) to 100
(very sure) successfully used in previous studies (Koriat,
Ackerman, Lockl, & Schneider, 2009; Koriat &
Shitzer-Reichert, 2002).
In the
recognition phase, word pairs of each type (i.e., either
identical, recombined, or new) were presented. Participants
had to indicate by opting for “yes” or “no” whether they
thought that the item had appeared in exactly this
combination in the studying phase. After that, the item
disappeared from the screen in order to avoid distraction
from the presented item pair, and participants were asked to
indicate how they generated the yes-or-no-decision. They had
to decide between three options: a) “I can remember the word
pair very well” (Recollection), b) “The word pair seems
familiar to me” (Familiarity), c) “I cannot remember the
word pair at all” (No memory). Finally, subjects had to
indicate on a hot-cold-scale equivalent to that used for
JOLs how sure they were that the given answer was correct
(CJ).
At the
end of the session, the test administrator asked whether the
subjects in the experimental condition had applied the
strategy while memorizing the items. Participants in the
control group were asked whether they had employed any
strategy, and if so, to specify the strategies and to
indicate how often they were used.
3. Results
A
preliminary analysis assessing the effect of gender did not
reveal any systematic differences between male and female
participants. Thus data were collapsed across this variable.
Scheffé tests were used as a post-hoc follow-up on main
effects. The level of significance was set to p < 0.05.
In a
first step of analysis, we assessed memory performance in
terms of the percentage of correctly recognized items, that
is, either identical items correctly recognized as “old” or
recombined or completely new items correctly classified as
“new” items. Next, we analyzed JOLs and CJs as indicators of
metacognitive monitoring. Finally, we will report changes in
the RFN judgments as cues for monitoring processes. Results
are reported as a function of age group and experimental
condition in order to examine the influence of cognitive
development and strategy instruction on recognition rates
and metacognitive monitoring.
3.1 Recognition
rates
The
first column of Table 1 shows the mean proportion of
correctly recognized items as a function of age group and
experimental condition, that is overall recognition rates.
An ANOVA with age group and experimental condition as
between-subject factors revealed a main effect of age group
(F(3,152) = 5.34;
p < .01; η2
= .10). A post-hoc analysis indicated that younger adults
performed significantly better than children (.80 vs. .71
correct, respectively). Furthermore, a main effect of the
experimental condition was found (F(1,152) = 21.82; p < .001; η2
= .13), indicating that those participants who had received
the strategy instruction recognized significantly more word
pairs correctly than participants who had not received such
an instruction (.80 vs. .72, respectively).
The
second to fourth column of Table 1 splits the recognition
rates into percentages depending on the type of word pair:
that is, whether the word pair in the recognition phase was
identical to that in the study phase or whether it was
recombined or a completely new word pair. Inferential
statistics were conducted separately for each word pair in
order to facilitate the interpretation. For the identical
word pairs, an ANOVA with age group and experimental
condition as between-subject factor revealed a significant
main effect of age group (F(3,152) = 5.14; p < .01; η2
= .09). A post-hoc analysis showed that children (.65)
recognized fewer of the identical items correctly than
younger (.77) and older adults (.76). Furthermore, the main
effect of the experimental condition reached significance (F(1,152) = 5.24; p < .05; η2
= .03) with subjects in the experimental group (.74)
recognizing more items correctly than subjects in the
control group (.69). For the recombined word pairs, only the
main effect of strategy instruction reached the significance
level (F(1,152) =
5.24; p <
.001; η2 = .11), with subjects in the strategy
instruction group recognizing more of the recombined word
pairs correctly (.75) than subjects who had not received the
strategy instruction (.61). As for the new word pairs, a
significant main effect of strategy instruction was found as
well (F(1,152) =
7.04; p < .01;
η2 = .04). Those participants who had received
the strategy instruction recognized more of the new word
pairs correctly (.94) than those who had not been thus
instructed (.88).
Table 1
Recognition
rates as a function of age group, experimental condition,
and type of word pair
Age
group |
Type of word pair |
|||
|
Overall |
Identic word pairs |
Recombined word pairs |
New word pairs |
Children Control
group Experimental
group |
.65
(.06) .77
(.11) |
.60
(.16) .71
(.18) |
.54
(.19) .72
(20) |
.88
(.13) .91
(.10) |
Adolescents Control
group Experimental
group |
.70
(.11) .81
(.09) |
.64
(.17) .76
(.12) |
.63
(.20) .76
(.15) |
.89
(.11) .94
(.09) |
Younger
Adults Control
group Experimental
group |
.79
(.09) .81
(.12) |
.75
(.10) .79
(.15) |
.73
(.19) .75
(.23) |
.92
(.10) .93
(.10) |
Older
Adults Control
group Experimental
group |
.74
(.12) .80
(.11) |
.79
(.14) .73
(.18) |
.53
(.28) .77
(.22) |
.85
(.21) .95
(.08) |
Standard
deviations are in parentheses.
3.2 Metacognitive
Monitoring
3.2.1
Mean JOLs
before correct vs. incorrect responses
Figure
1 shows participants’ mean JOL ratings as a function of the
correctness of the subsequent response, age group, and
experimental condition. An ANOVA with correctness of
response as within-subject factor and age group and
experimental condition as between-subject factors revealed a
significant main effect of correctness of response (F(1,151) = 135.38;
p < .001; η2
= .47): Subjects gave higher JOLs before correct (57.98)
than before incorrect responses (46.78). In addition, a
significant interaction between the factors correctness of
response and experimental condition was found (F(1,151) = 6.22; p < .05; η2
= .04). Furthermore, the triple interaction between
correctness of response, experimental condition, and age
group attained a significant level (F(3,151) = 3.43; p < .05; η2
= .06). In order to examine the direction of the
interactions post hoc, we analyzed the experimental and the
control group data separately. For subjects in the
experimental condition, an ANOVA with correctness of
response as within-subject factor and age group as
between-subject factor revealed a main effect of correctness
of response (F(1,75)
= 89.07; p <
.001; η2 = .54) with mean JOLs being higher
before correct (59.58) than before incorrect responses
(45.98). For the participants in the control condition the
main effect of correctness of response was also significant
(F(1,76) = 47.36;
p < .001; η2
= .38). Furthermore, for the subjects in the control
condition a significant interaction between correctness of
response and age group was found (F(3,76) = 5.93; p < .01; η2
= .19). Subsequent analyses revealed that only the
adolescents and the younger adults distinguished between
correct and incorrect responses given that it was only for
these two age groups that the factor correctness of response
turned out to be significant (children: F(1,19) = 0.66; p = .426; η2
= .03; adolescents: F(1,19)
= 19.82; p <
.001; η2 = .51; younger adults: F(1,19) = 66.06; p < .001; η2
= .78; older adults: F(1,19)
= 4.43; p = .05;
η2 = .19).
Figure 1.
Mean JOLs preceding correct vs. incorrect answers as a
function of age group and experimental condition. (see pdf)
3.2.2
JOL
accuracy
In
order to assess JOL accuracy as a function of age group,
question format and experimental condition, Goodman-Kruskal
gamma correlations between JOLs and recall performance were
computed for each participant, and then averaged for each
single cell in the experimental design. Gamma correlations
are considered to be the most appropriate measure of
metacognitive accuracy (Nelson, 1984) and are commonly used
in the contemporary literature (Nelson & Dunlosky, 1991;
Schneider et al., 2000). A positive correlation indicates
that higher JOLs were given for items that were recalled
correctly than for those recalled incorrectly.
Table
2 shows mean gamma correlations for JOLs as a function of
age group and experimental condition. One-tailed t-tests
revealed that all gamma correlations were different from
zero for almost all groups. The only exception concerned the
children in the control group whose mean gamma correlations
for JOLs were not significantly different from zero.
Table
2
Mean
gamma correlations as a function of age group and
experimental condition
Age
group |
JOLs |
CJs |
Children Control
group Experimental
group |
.03
(.24) .32
(.28) |
.18
(.20) .38
(.29) |
Adolescents Control
group Experimental
group |
.20
(.19) .30
(.19) |
.33
(.21) .42
(.21) |
Younger Adults Control
group Experimental
group |
.32
(.15) .27
(.23) |
.42
(.20) .35
(.23) |
Older Adults Control
group Experimental
group |
.14
(.26) .29
(.25) |
.38
(.26) .49
(.26) |
Standard
deviations are in parentheses.
An
ANOVA with age group and experimental condition as
between-subject factors revealed a significant main effect
of strategy instruction (F(1,151) = 11.36; p < .001; η2
= .07). In addition, the ANOVA showed a significant
interaction between age group and experimental condition (F(3,151) = 3.58; p < .05; η2
= .07). In order to examine the direction of this effect,
the mean gamma correlations of each age group were tested
individually using univariate ANOVAs. In children, the main
effect of experimental condition was significant (F(1,38) = 12.25; p < .01; η2
= .24) with children in strategy-instruction group having
higher gamma correlations (.32) than those in the control
group (.03). In older adults, the results pointed into the
same direction (experimental group: 29; control group: .14):
the main effect of experimental condition was just short of
being significant (F(1,38)
= 3.29; p = .077;
η2 = .08).
3.2.3
Mean CJs
after correct vs. incorrect responses
Differentiation
in
CJs was analyzed in the same way as for JOLs (cf. figure 2):
mean CJ ratings after correct and incorrect responses
respectively were calculated for each subject. As for JOLs,
an ANOVA with correctness of response as within-subject
factor, and age group and experimental condition as
between-subject factors was conducted. The ANOVA revealed a
main effect of age group (F(3,151) = 6.26; p < .001; η2
= .11). Post-hoc tests according to Scheffé’s procedure
showed that younger adults gave significantly lower mean CJs
(65.56) than the other age groups (children: 76.10,
adolescents: 74.59, older adults: 75.00), regardless whether
the answer was correct or not. In addition, a main effect of
correctness of response was found (F(3,151) = 188.75;
p < .001; η2
= .56). Subjects of all age groups gave higher ratings after
correct (79.02) than after incorrect responses (66.60).
Finally, the interaction between correctness of response and
age group reached the significance level (F(3,151) = 5.61; p < .001; η2
= .10). To further explore the direction of the effect,
separate ANOVAs for CJs after correct and incorrect answers
were conducted. For correct answers, the main effect of age
group reached significance (F(3,156) = 3.42; p < .05; η2
= .06). Subsequent post-hoc analyses showed that older
adults (82.57) were more confident after correct responses
than younger adults (74.25). For the CJs after incorrect
answers, a significant main effect of age group was found as
well (F(3,155) =
7.89; p <
.001; η2 = .13). Here, post-hoc analyses showed
that younger adults gave lower CJs (57.33) after incorrect
responses than the other three age groups (children: 72.63;
adolescents: 69.00; older adults: 67.42).
Figure 2. Mean CJs
after correct vs. incorrect answers as a function of age
group and experimental condition (see pdf)
3.2.4
CJ
accuracy
CJ
accuracy was assessed in the same way as JOL accuracy. Mean
gamma correlations are displayed in Table 2. All gamma
correlations were different from zero (using one-tailed
t-tests). An ANOVA with age group and experimental condition
as between-subject factors revealed a main effect of age
group (F(3,151) =
2.93; p <
.05; η2 = .06). Post-hoc tests according to
Scheffé showed a significant difference between children
(.28) and older adults (.44). Furthermore, a main effect of
strategy instruction was found (F(1,151) = 4.75; p < .05; η2
= .03). Subjects in the strategy instruction condition had
higher mean gamma correlations (.41) than participants
without such instruction (.33).
3.3 RFN-judgments
In
addition, the RFN-judgments subjects made were contrasted.
We compared the percentage of how often each option was
picked out because age groups differed in quantity of items.
Table 3 shows the percentage of each RFN-judgment as a
function of age group and experimental condition. An ANOVA
was conducted with RFN-judgment as within-subject factor and
age group and experimental condition as between-subject
factor. We found a significant main effect of RFN-judgment (F(1,152) = 29.99; p < .001; η2
= .17). Paired contrasts revealed that subjects chose
“familiarity” (.22) less often than “recollection” (.40) and
“no memory” (.38). Furthermore a significant interaction
between RFN-judgment and experimental condition was found (F(1,152) = 4.12; p < .05; η2
= .03). Separate ANOVAs for each judgment showed that the
strategy instruction had a significant effect only for the
answer “familiarity” (F(1,158)
= 12.63; p <
.01; η2 = .07), and “no memory” (F(1,158) = 5.54; p < .05; η2
= .03): subjects in the experimental group chose the
“familiarity” option less often (.19) than subjects in the
control condition (.26), and the “no memory” option more
often (.41) than the control group (.34).
Table 3
RFN-judgment
in
percentage of chosen option as a function of age group and
experimental condition
Age
group |
Recollection |
Familiarity |
No
memory |
Children Control
group Experimental
group |
.41
(.24) .35
(.23) |
.27
(.15) .19
(.11) |
.32
(.25) .45
(.21) |
Adolescents Control
group Experimental
group |
.41
(.19) .42
(.18) |
.30
(.16) .20
(.11) |
.29
(.18) .38
(.20) |
Younger
Adults Control
group Experimental
group |
.35
(.16) .41
(.22) |
.24
(.10) .22
(.11) |
.40
(.17) .38
(.17) |
Older
Adults Control
group Experimental
group |
.42
(.21) .40
(.20) |
.22
(.11) .15
(.09) |
.36
(.16) .45
(.22) |
Standard
deviations are in parentheses.
3.4 Spontaneous
and instructed strategy use
In a
last step of analysis, we assessed the outcomes for the
Strategy Use Questionnaire. Table 4 shows how many
participants of each age group and experimental condition
reported having used the visual imagery strategy.
Participants’ open responses were categorized as visual
imagery by two independent raters (kappa = .95).
We
compared the use of the visual imagery strategy in the
experimental and the control group. An ANOVA with age group
and experimental condition as between-subject factors
revealed a significant main effect of age group (F(3,154) = 10.97; p < .001; η2
= .18) with post-hoc analysis showing that younger adults
(.83) applied mental imagery more often than the other age
groups (children: .48; adolescents: .53; older adults: .55).
Furthermore, we found a significant main effect of
experimental condition (F(1,154) = 221.46; p < .001; η2
= .63): Participants who received the strategy instruction
used mental imagery much more often than participants in the
control condition (.28 vs. .95). The interaction between age
group and experimental condition was also significant (F(3,154) = 9.38; p < .001; η2
= .16). Separate analyses carried out for participants in
the experimental group on the one hand and participants in
the control group on the other hand showed there was no main
effect of age group for participants who received the
strategy instruction, indicating that the participants were
able to transfer the training to the learning process. For
subjects in the control group, the ANOVA revealed a main
effect of age group (F(3,74)
= 12.30; p <
.001; η2 = .35).
A post-hoc analysis showed that the percentage of
younger adults (.65) spontaneously applying a mental imagery
strategy was significantly higher than that of the other age
groups. More specifically, none of the children, only 5% of
the adolescents, and about 25% of the older adults applied
such a strategy spontaneously.
Table
4
Percentage of
subjects reporting the use of visual imagery as a function
of age group and experimental condition
Age
group |
Children |
Adolescents |
Younger
Adults |
Older
Adults |
Control
group Experimental
group |
.00
.95
|
.05
1.00 |
.65 1.00 |
.25 .85 |
4. Discussion
The
present study is among the first to explore metacognitive
monitoring skills across the life-span, and also to
investigate the effects of a memory strategy training
principally suited to improve skills in this domain. Thus
our study combined traditional interests of the
developmental and cognitive literature on metacognition. In
particular, we focused on possible positive effects of
strengthening highly diagnostic cues (Koriat, 1997). This
was achieved by training half of our participants in visual
imagery before memorizing item pairs, and by assessing the
training effects on monitoring quality, that is, JOL and CJ
differentiation and accuracy. We postulated that instructing
subjects to connect idiosyncratic content to items should
lead them to rely less on familiarity but to focus on
recollection processes when monitoring their performance. An
important innovative aspect of our study was its life-span
perspective in that four age groups (children in third
grade, adolescents in seventh and eighth grade, younger and
older adults) were included in the sample. Especially for
children only very few studies exist that have explored the
cues underlying monitoring judgments but for all included
age-groups more research on the effects of familiarity and
recollection-based cues is needed.
First,
the results show that our manipulation of task difficulty
across the age groups was successful: recognition rates in
both the experimental and the control condition were of
comparable height across the four age groups. Thus, the task
was suitable for participants from primary school to older
adulthood. Secondly, we found significant gains in
recognition performance in subjects who underwent the
strategy training. This effect was most pronounced for
recombined and new item pairs but still substantial for the
overall data. These results thus point to the fact that our
experimental manipulation was successful and are in line
with many previous findings: It has been shown for various
age groups from primary school to older adulthood that
visual imagery is an efficient strategy in paired-associate
learning (Richardson, 1998; Verhaegen et al., 1992), and
that its instruction leads to superior recall performance
compared to spontaneous use (Shing et al., 2009). Also in
accordance with the literature, self-reported use of visual
imagery was higher in the experimental than in the control
group, and substantial spontaneous use of visual imagery was
only reported by young adults.
We
acknowledge that we cannot rule out the possibility that
pre-training differences had an effect on memory
performance. Yet, participants in our study were randomly
assigned to experimental and control group which
substantially reduces the risk of imbalance in potentially
confounding factors. Although our sample was at the lower
end of recommended sample sizes for randomization (Bortz
& Döring, 2009), the expected positive results of the
strategy training on recognition performance and the
reported low level of strategy use in the control group
speak for a true effect of the strategy training. However,
in future research the inclusion of a pre-training
measurement would further substantiate the results.
In
accord with our hypothesis, we found evidence that both JOL
differentiation between later correct and incorrect answers
and JOL accuracy as measured by Goodman-Kruskal gamma
correlations were enhanced by our strategy training in
certain age groups. Concerning JOL differentiation, in the
experimental group subjects of all age groups differentiated
between correct and incorrect answers compared to the
control group where only adolescents and younger adults gave
higher JOLs before correct than before incorrect responses.
Additionally, adolescents in the experimental condition
descriptively showed a more pronounced discrimination
between later correct and incorrect answers, as compared to
those in the control condition. Concerning JOL accuracy, the
analysis revealed that only children’s accuracy improved
significantly by the strategy instruction. Although there
was a similar tendency in the group of older adults, only a
marginal effect of strategy training was found. Similarly,
adolescents’ and younger adults’ gamma correlations were not
enhanced by the training.
We
also detected the expected positive effect of visual imagery
on CJ quality. For CJ accuracy, we found a significant main
effect of strategy instruction, implying that all age groups
benefitted from the strategy instruction. Concerning
differentiation, training effects were found for children
and older adults; here, the difference between correct and
incorrect answers was about twice as high in the
experimental condition than in the control condition. In
contrast, adolescents and younger adults showed about the
same amount of differentiation in both conditions.
The
strategy training had positive effects on both JOLs and CJs
which were most pronounced for children and older adults for
both monitoring indicators. Yet, impact on CJs could be
detected in a broader age range than for JOLs. This points
to the fact that both JOLs and CJs rely on cues involved in
our strategy instruction but at the same time draw onto
different sources. For CJs information from the retrieval
process might be most significant and it is possible that
cues based on recollection processes are even more important
for accurate judgments than for JOLs. Further research is
needed to clarify this point.
In
sum, the results confirm our predictions that a strategy
training can improve the quality of metamemory monitoring
judgments. This finding is in line with outcomes of other
studies that have shown a positive influence of strategy use
on prospective and retrospective monitoring judgments for
children, younger and older adults (Hertzog et al., 2010;
Nietfield & Schraw, 2002; Robinson et al., 2006; Shing
et al., 2009) and expands the existing literature by
training strategy use explicitly and by the inclusion of two
monitoring judgments.
The
developmental trends found in our study were also as
expected: That is, children and older adults benefitted most
from strategy instruction in terms of enhancing both their
JOL and CJ quality, followed by the adolescents. These
results are in accordance with developmental trends in
regard to production deficits concerning strategy use
(Naveh-Benjamin et al., 2007), and of recollection processes
in general (Ghetti & Angelini, 2008). This outcome also
emphasizes the practicability of our training. It proved to
be effective yet was simple enough to be understood by
elementary school children, and could also be successfully
acquired by older adults in very short time.
Still,
the strategy training did not account for much monitoring
improvements in younger adults, with the exception of CJ
accuracy. One possible explanation for this finding is the
high level of spontaneous use of visual imagery reported by
young adults in the control group. It appears likely that
the short strategy training did not greatly improve the
already high level of strategy use in young adults.
Obviously, young adults showed high competence to memorize
and to monitor their recall performance in paired-associate
learning without further instruction. It is possible and
should be investigated in further research that more
pronounced effects of a strategy training would be found on
more complex tasks. Support for this assumption comes from
studies where gains from a strategy training in CJ accuracy
could be shown for a comprehensive problem-solving task
(Nietfield & Schraw, 2002).
Presumably
further reasons were responsible for the fact that we were
not able to confirm the positive effects of the strategy
instruction for all age groups and for all indicators of
metacognitive monitoring. One possible cause is the
influence of the memory paradigm. A recognition task was
chosen in this study in order to explore the basis of memory
and monitoring processes by collecting RFN-judgments.
Specifically, participants were asked to rate the quality of
their recognition memory as recollection, familiarity or no
memory as an indication of the mode of action of our
strategy training. Yet, it seems possible that recognition
processes make differentiation of recollection and
familiarity-based cues more difficult than recall, given
that no active memory retrieval was necessary. Recall
processes seem to offer more cues to increase the accuracy
of monitoring judgments, as compared to recognition (Buratti
& Allwood, 2012). Other research showed a positive
effect of strategy use on monitoring accuracy required
active memory recall (Hertzog et al., 2010; Robinson et al.,
2006). Although Shing and colleagues (2009) found positive
effects of a strategy training on CJs in a recognition task,
they used more complicated stimuli (Malay word pairs) than
those used in this study. Furthermore they collected
metacognitive measures of calibration and not resolution as
done here.
Another
explanation for this unexpected outcome could be that we
collected delayed JOLs which have been shown to be more
accurate than immediate JOLs (Nelson & Dunlosky, 1991).
This seems to be due to the fact that delayed JOLs in all
age groups are based on active assessment of long-term
memory (recollection processes) instead of short-term and
long-term memory as in immediate JOLs. Yet, we wanted to
explore a possible add-on effect to maximize the quality of
monitoring judgments. This in turn makes it more difficult
to show an effect than with immediate JOLs which are
commonly used in many studies (Daniels et al., 2009;
Hertzog, Fulton, Mandiwala, & Dunlosky, 2013; Robinson
et al., 2006).
A
third possibility is that the effects of a short strategy
intervention as used here are generally limited. Possibly
results of a longer intervention could exceed the promising
findings of our training in all age groups and for both
monitoring indicators. This issue would be worth to be
explored in a follow-up study.
Yet in
general, a strategy instruction proved to be a promising
starting point to influence monitoring processes in
different age groups and across various monitoring
indicators. As a mode of operation we proposed that the
strategy instruction should be effective due to a shift in
accessible cues. Specifically, we assumed that improvement
should be due to the fact that now sources of monitoring
judgments should be less familiarity-based cues and
increasingly recollection-based cues. RFN-judgments confirm
that - as predicted - the number of familiarity based
judgments was significantly reduced in the experimental
group. This trend was accompanied by more “no memory”
responses. We assume that subjects in the experimental group
profited from the instruction in that they were able to
decide for which memories no reliable cues existed. In such
cases, the answer “no memory” was correctly given.
Participants could have used recall of interactive imagery
to discriminate recollection states: If they can recall
something about the image created to memorize a word pair,
they are more confident that they base their delayed-JOL on
a diagnostic cue. Strategy recall seems to have a similar
effect on Feeling-of-knowing judgments (Hertzog, Fulton,
Sinclair, & Dunlosky, 2014). Thus, although the number
of recollection-based memories as perceived by the
participants could not be enhanced, the strategy training
seems to have increased participants’ awareness of possible
cues and enabled them to distinguish more securely between
real memories and no memories. This contrast of correct
recall and no retrieval at the time of the JOL has been
shown to be the most important source for high accuracy of
delayed-JOLs (Nelson et al., 2004). It appears likely that
the number of guesses which probably fell into the
familiarity category could be successfully reduced by our
training. Given that no age effects were found, the
instruction seems to be effective in a similar way across
all age groups in that it reduces the impact of familiarity.
In
sum, the present study yields evidence that a strategy
training is a suitable means to improve prospective and
retrospective monitoring processes throughout the life-span,
especially in children and older adults. The instruction
used in this study proved to be an economic procedure that
could be successfully applied in different age groups. The
training was simple enough to be easily mastered by both
elementary school children and older adults. Therefore a
transfer to everyday-life situations seems possible.
The
findings of the present study emphasize the significance of
recollection-based cues as well as its distinction from
other cues for metacognitive monitoring processes and
encourage further research in this direction. Especially an
expansion of our findings to more complex stimuli like texts
or films and the investigation of the effect of a more
elaborate strategy training are of interest. This would
allow to further
test relevance for every-day life. Additionally, it would be
interesting to explore the effects of a strategy training on
a larger samples as our study included relative small sample
sizes per group and to follow up long-term effects of the
training. We do not exclude the possibility that multiple
cues underlie and can significantly influence monitoring
processing. Yet, along with recent research (Ghetti et al.,
2008; McCabe & Soderstrom, 2011; Metcalfe & Finn,
2008; Toth et al., 2011) we assume that exploring the role
of recollection and familiarity processes in mediating the
accuracy of monitoring judgments is a promising issue for
future research.
Improving
monitoring processes is of great importance as it is very
closely linked to memory performance. Thus successful
monitoring represents a very valuable competence in
different learning contexts. Our results demonstrate that
monitoring occurs from childhood on, but that there is still
room for improvement at every age level. At the same time,
the findings also illustrate that there are very economical
ways to improve metacognitive monitoring in different
age-groups. They thus indicate a direction which is worth to
be pursued in future research.
Keypoints
Approaches to improve
metacognitive monitoring in a broad age range covering the
life-span are still very rare.
Integration of
traditional developmental and cognitive research questions, as
in this study, are scarce in the metacognitive literature.
Our results show that
a short training in visual imagery enhances both memory and
metamemory performance, especially in children and older
adults.
Improvements in
monitoring seem to be associated to a use of more reliable
cues after the strategy training.
Acknowledgments
This research was conducted as part of a research
project on the development of procedural metacognitive
knowledge across the life-span and financed by the German
Research Foundation (DFG-Gz. SCHN 315/45-1). We wish to thank
all participating children, adolescents and adults as well as
teachers, principals and parents for their cooperation.
References
Begg, I.,
Duft, S., Lalonde, P., Melnick, R., & Sanvito, J. (1989).
Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal
of Memory and Language, 28, 610-632. DOI:
10.1016/0749-596X(89)90016-8
Benjamin, A.
S., & Bjork, R. A. (1996). Retrieval fluency as a
metacognitive index. In L. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory
and metacognition (pp. 309-338). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.55
Bortz, J., &
Döring, N. (2009). Forschungsmethoden
und Evaluation für Human- und Sozialwissenschaftler. Heidelberg:
Springer.
Buratti,
S., & Allwood, C. M. (2012). The accuracy of
meta-metacognitive judgments: Regulating the realism of
confidence. Cognitive Processing, 13, 243-253. DOI:
10.1007/s10339-012-0440-5
Connor, L.
T., Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (1997). Age-related
differences in absolute but not relative metamemory accuracy.
Psychology and Aging, 12 (1), 50-71. DOI:
10.1037/0882-7974.12.1.50
Daniels, K.
A., Toth, J. P., & Hertzog, C. (2009). Aging and
recollection in the accuracy of judgments of learning. Psychology
and Aging, 24, 494-500. DOI: 10.1037/a0015269
Dunlosky,
J., & Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognition. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Flavell, J.
H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children’s knowledge about
the mind. Annual Review
of Psychology, 50, 21-45. DOI:
10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.21
Ghetti, S.,
& Angelini, L. (2008). The development of recollection and
familiarity in childhood and adolescence: Evidence from the
dual-process signal detection model. Child Development, 79,
339-358. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01129.x
Ghetti, S.,
Lyons, K. E., Lazzarin, F., & Cornoldi, C. (2008). The
development of metamemory monitoring during retrieval: The
case of memory strength and memory absence. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 99, 157-181. DOI:
10.1016/j.jecp.2007.11.001
Hertzog, C.,
Fulton, E. K., Mandviwala, L., & Dunlosky, J. (2013).
Older adults show deficits in retrieving and decoding
associative mediators generated at study. Developmental
Psychology, 49, 1127-1131. DOI: 10.1037/a0029414
Hertzog, C.,
Fulton, E. K., Sinclair, S. M., & Dunlosky, J. (2014).
Recalled aspects of original encoding strategies influence
episodic feelings of knowing. Memory and Cognition, 42,
126-140. DOI: 10.3758/s13421-013-0348-z
Hertzog, C.,
Sinclair, S. M., & Dunlosky, J. (2010). Age differences in
the monitoring of learning: Cross-sectional evidence of spared
resolution across the adult life span. Developmental
Psychology, 46, 939-948. DOI: 10.1037/a0019812
Holmes, A. E., & Weaver III, C. A. (2010).
Eyewitness memory and misinformation: Are remember/know
judgments more reliable than subjective confidence? Applied
Psychology in Criminal Justice, 6, 47-61.
Kelley, C.
M. & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Adult egocentrism: Subjective
experience versus analytic bases for judgment. Journal of Memory and
Language, 35, 157-175. DOI: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0009
Kelley, C.
M., & Sahakyan, L. (2003). Memory, monitoring, and control
in the attainment of memory accuracy. Journal of Memory
and Language, 48, 704-721. DOI:
10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00504-1
Koriat, A.
(1997). Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: A
cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349-370. DOI:
10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349
Koriat, A.
(2012). The Self-Consistency Model of Subjective Confidence. Psychological Review, 119,
80-113. DOI: 10.1037/a0025648
Koriat, A., Ackerman,
R., Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). The memorizing effort
heuristic in judgments of learning: A developmental perspective.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 265-279.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.10.005
Koriat, A., &
Levy-Sadot, R. (1999). Processes underlying metacognitive
judgments: Information-based and experience-based monitoring of
one's own knowledge. In S. Chaiken, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in
social psychology (pp. 483-502). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Koriat, A.,
& Shitzer-Reichert, R. (2002). Metacognitive judgments and
their accuracy: Insights from the processes underlying
judgments of learning in children. In M. Izaute, P. Chambres,
P.-J. Marescaux (Eds.), Metacognition: Process, function, and use (pp.
1-17). New York: Kluwer.
Leonesio, R.
J., & Nelson, T. O. (1990). Do different metamemory
judgments tap the same underlying aspects of memory? Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 16, 464-470. DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.464
Matvey, G.,
Dunlosky, J., & Guttentag, R. (2001). Fluency of retrieval
at study affects judgments of learning (JOLs): An analytic or
nonanalytic basis for JOLs? Memory & Cognition,
29, 222-233. DOI: 10.3758/bf03194916
McCabe, D.
P., & Soderstrom, N. C. (2011). Recollection-based
prospective metamemory judgments are more accurate than those
based on confidence: Judgments of remembering and knowing
(JORKS). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140,
605-621. DOI: 10.1037/a0024014
Metcalfe,
J., & Finn, B. (2008). Familiarity and retrieval processes
in delayed judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, Learning, Memory & Cognition, 34,
1084-1097. DOI: 10.1037/a0012580
Naveh-Benjamin,
M.,
Brav, T. K., & Levy, O. (2007). The associative memory
deficit of older adults: The role of strategy utilization. Psychology
and Aging, 22, 202-208. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.1.202
Nelson, T.
O. (1984). A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of
Feeling-of-Knowing predictions. Psychological Bulletin, 95,
109-133. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.109
Nelson, T.
O., & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people's judgments of
learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting
subsequent recall: The "Delayed-JOL Effect". Psychological
Science, 2, 267-270. DOI:
10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00147.x
Nelson, T.
O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical
framework and new findings. In G. Bower (Ed.), The
psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research
and theory (Vol. 26, pp. 125-141). New York: Academic
Press.
Nelson, T.
O., Narens, L., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). A revised
methodology for research on metamemory: Pre-judgment recall
and monitoring (PRAM). Psychological
Methods, 9, 53-69. DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.1.53
Nietfeld, J.
L., & Schraw, G. (2002). The effect of knowledge and
strategy training on monitoring accuracy. The Journal of
Educational Research, 95(3),
131-142. DOI: 10.1080/00220670209596583
Pressley,
M., & Levin, J. R. (1977). Developmental differences in
subjects' associative-learning strategies and performance:
Assessing a hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 24, 431-439. DOI:
10.1016/0022-0965(77)90089-3
Rhodes, M.
G., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Metacognition and part-set
cuing: Can inference be predicted at retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 36,
1429-1438. DOI: 10.3758/MC.36.8.1429
Richardson,
J. T. E. (1998). The availability and effectiveness of
reported mediators in associative learning: A historical
review and an experimental investigation. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 5, 597-614. DOI:
10.3758/BF03208837
Robinson, A.
E., Hertzog, C., & Dunlosky, J. (2006). Aging, encoding
fluency, and metacognitive monitoring. Aging,
Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13(3-4), 458-478. DOI:
10.1080/13825580600572983
Robinson, M.
D., Johnson, J. T., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Process
versus content in eyewitness metamemory monitoring. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6(3), 207-221. DOI:
10.1037/1076-898X.6.3.207
Roderer, T.,
& Roebers, C. M. (2010). Explicit and implicit confidence
judgments and developmental differences in metamemory: An
eye-tracking approach. Metacognition
and Learning, 5, 229-250. DOI: 10.1007/s11409-010-9059-z
Roebers, C. M. (2002). Confidence judgments in
children’s and adults’ event recall and suggestibility. Developmental
Psychology, 38, 1052-1067. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.1052
Roebers, C. M., von der
Linden, N., Schneider, W., & Howie, P. (2007). Children’s metamemorial
judgments in an event recall task. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 97, 117 - 137. DOI:
10.1016/j.jecp.2006.12.006
Schneider,
W. (2010). Metacognition and memory development in childhood
and adolescence. In H. S. Waters & W. Schneider (Eds.), Metacognition, strategy
use, and instruction (pp. 54-81). New York: Guilford
Press.
Schneider, W. (2015). Memory development
from early childhood through emerging adulthood. New York: Springer.
Schneider,
W., Visé, M., Lockl, K., & Nelson, T. O. (2000).
Developmental trends in children's memory monitoring: Evidence
from a judgment-of-learning (JOL) task. Cognitive
Development, 15, 115-134. DOI:
10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00024-1
Shing, Y.
L., Werkle-Bergner, M., Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U.
(2009). Committing memory errors with high confidence: Older
adults do but children don't. Memory, 17, 169-179.
DOI: 10.1080/09658210802190596
Son, L. K.,
& Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control
strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26,
204-221. DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.1.204
Souchay, C.,
Bacon, E., & Danion, J. (2006). Metamemory in
schizophrenia: An exploration of the feeling-of-knowing state.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 828-840.
DOI: 10.1080/13803390591000846
Toth, J. P.,
Daniels, K. A., & Solinger, L. A. (2011). What you know
can hurt you: Effects of age and prior knowledge on JOL
accuracy. Psychology
and Aging, 26, 919-931. DOI: 10.1037/a0023379
Touron, D.
R., Hertzog, C., & Speagle, J. Z. (2010). Subjective
learning discounts test type: Evidence from an associative
learning and transfer task. Experimental Psychology, 327-337.
DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000039
Verhaeghen,
P., Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1992). Improving memory
performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A
meta-analytic study. Psychology and
Aging, 7, 242-251. DOI:
10.1037/0882-7974.7.2.242
Weinert, F. E., &
Schneider, W. (1996). Entwicklung des Gedächtnisses . In D.
Albert & K.-H. Stapf (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der
Psychologie, Themenbereich C, Serie II, Band 4 (S.
433-487). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Willoughby,
T., Porter, L., Belsito, L., & Yearsley, T. (1999). Use of
elaboration strategies by students in grades two, four and
six. The Elementary School Journal, 99(3), 221-231.
Wong, J. T.,
Cramer, S. J, & Gallo, D. A. (2012). Age-related reduction
of the confidence-accuracy relationship in episodic memory:
effects of recollection quality and retrieval monitoring. Psychology and Aging, 27(4),
1053-1065. DOI: 10.1037/a0027686
Zakay, D.,
& Tuvia, R. (1998). Choice latency times as determinants
of post-decisional confidence. Acta Psychologica, 98,
103-115. DOI: 10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00037-1
[1] Corresponding author: Nicole von der
Linden, Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg,
Röntgenring 10, 97070 Würzburg, Germany. Phone: +49(0)931
/ 31 89067, Fax: +49(0)931 / 31 2763, Email:
linden@psychologie.uni-wuerzburg.de
DOI:
http://dx.doi.org/10.14786/flr.v3i4.196