University teachers’ perceptions of
appropriate emotion display and high-quality
teacher-student relationship: Similarities and differences
across cultural-educational contexts
Gerda Hagenauera[1], Michaela Gläser-Zikudab, &
Simone E. Voletc
aUniversity of Bern, Switzerland
bUniversity of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany
cMurdoch University, Australia
Article received 27 January / revised 8 April / accepted
27 April / available online 12 May
Abstract
Research on teachers’
emotion display and the quality of the teacher-student
relationship in higher education is increasingly
significant in the context of rapidly developing
internationalization in higher education, with scholars
(and students) moving across countries for research and
teaching. However, there is little theoretically
grounded empirical research in this area, and the
different research strands remain relatively unconnected.
The present study aimed to address this gap.
Psychological, educational and cross-cultural theories
were brought together to investigate the interplay of
emotion display and the quality of the teacher-student
relationship from a teachers’ perspective and across
“cultural-educational” contexts. Given that social
interaction, and the mores and norms associated with
emotion display are often culturally underpinned, this study explored how university teachers
in two so-called “individualistic” countries with
different educational systems displayed positive and
negative emotions in their teaching and what they
perceived as an ideal teacher-student relationship.
Australian (N = 15) and German (N = 9) university teachers
in teacher education were interviewed. The study revealed that
while both groups viewed the open expression of
positive emotions as integral to teaching, and negative
emotions to be controlled based on their understanding of
professionalism, significant group differences were also
found. While the Australian teacher educators reported
higher and more intense expression of positive emotions,
their German counterparts reported more open anger
display. Subtle yet noteworthy differences in the TSR
quality between the two groups of teachers emerged. The
findings of this study have implications for research and
practice in international higher education.
Keywords: teacher emotions, emotion display, higher
education, teacher-student relationship, cross-cultural
comparison, internationalization of higher education
[1] Corresponding author: Gerda Hagenauer;
University of Bern, Institute of Educational Science,
Fabrikstrasse 8, 3012 Bern, Switzerland; email: gerda.hagenauer@edu.unibe.ch Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.14786/flr.v4i3.236
1.
Introduction
Emotions
in teaching have received increased attention in educational
research in the school context (Newberry, Gallant, &
Riley, 2013; Schutz & Zembylas, 2011, Schutz, 2014).
Teachers’ experience of emotions and the communication of
these emotions are expected to have a significant impact on
the quality of their teaching practice and the
socio-emotional climate in the classroom (Jennings &
Greenberg, 2009). While the literature on teacher emotions
at school steadily increases, affective factors in
teaching-learning-processes remain largely neglected in the
higher education (HE) literature (Beard, Clegg, & Smith,
2004; Quinlan, 2016), in particular teacher emotions (Moore
& Kuol, 2007). However, recent empirical research has
shown that teaching is also experienced emotionally in HE
(Hagenauer & Volet, 2014a; Postareff &
Lindblom-Ylänne, 2011), and is related to quality indicators
of teaching, e.g. student-centered teaching (Trigwell,
2012), that affect students’ learning (e.g., students’
engagement; Zhang & Zhang, 2013). Besides emotional
aspects, the teacher-student relationship (= TSR) in HE is
also a highly neglected field of research, in particular
from a HE teachers’ perspective (Hagenauer & Volet,
2014c; Walker & Gleaves, 2016). Some research has shown
that emotions and relationships are strongly intertwined
(e.g. Parkinson, Fischer, & Manstead, 2005): If someone
seeks to understand people’s emotions in interactions, the
quality of the relationship has to be considered as it
contributes to the quality of the emotion evoked. In turn,
the way emotions are communicated (= emotion display)
contributes to the development of relationships (Boiger
& Mesquita, 2012). Thus, research that combines the two
research strands – emotions / emotion display and the TSR –
is warranted in order to better understand HE
teacher-student interactions that form the basis of quality
teaching and learning processes.
The research
presented here follows an earlier study on Australian
university teachers’ emotions and emotion display (Hagenauer
& Volet, 2014a, b). The present research aimed to
increase understanding in this area through a cross-cultural
perspective, employing interview accounts of Australian and
German HE teachers, and by bringing in the aspect of the
TSR. Based on psychological research on cross-cultural
differences in emotions combined with a social-psychological
lens on emotions (Parkinson, Fischer, & Manstead, 2005),
we examined whether display modes of emotions differ between
teachers in an Australian and German HE context, if
differences in the quality of the TSR can be found between
countries and how emotion display and the TSR are linked.
Perceived differences in relationship quality may signal the
presence of differences in display modes. The focus is on a
particular group of teachers in HE, namely teacher
educators, who fulfill a special function as they do not
only teach content in their respective subject, but are also
expected to model teaching behaviour to their students
(Lunenberg, Korthagen, & Swennen, 2007). Thus, the way
university teacher educators relate to their students and
how they display their emotions, does not only affect the
teaching-learning-environment but also serves as a model for
future school-teaching practices of pre-service teacher
students.
The present study is innovative in three
aspects: Firstly, in a field of research with scarce
empirical evidence so far, it brings together in one study
psychological and educational theoretical strands of
research, which have traditionally been researched
separately, namely research on teacher emotions, emotion
display and the TSR in HE. The cross-cultural perspective
adds another dimension, which is important to explore in the
context of the internationalization of HE. Secondly, it
adopts the concept of “cultural-educational context” (Volet,
2001), and thus moves beyond the frequently made cultural
comparisons between collectivistic and individualistic
countries. Thirdly, it uses a qualitative approach to
investigate differences across cultural-education contexts.
This enables an in-depth exploration of cultural-educational
practices, which differs methodologically from the typically
quantitative driven approaches used in cross-cultural
emotion research (e.g. Safdar et al., 2009).
1.1
Emotion and emotion display in higher
education
From an appraisal
theoretical perspective (Ellsworth & Scherer, 2003) it
is assumed that emotions arise from the cognitive evaluation
of a situation (e.g., a teacher judges the learning
behaviour of a particular student). As evaluations of
situations vary across people, the same situation can
trigger different emotions in different people. According to
this approach, emotions only develop if a situation is of
relevance for people (= primary appraisal, Lazarus,
1999); otherwise people remain emotionally “untouched”.
Further appraisal cognitions, e.g. in terms of
controllability or goal attainment, determine the quality of
the respective emotion. Appraisals also influence whether
people decide to suppress or to show the particular emotion.
The ability to display emotions appropriately
is a competence that can be linked to teachers’ overall
emotion regulation competence (Gross, 2002). According to
Gross (2010, p. 497) emotion regulation “refers to how we
try to influence which emotions we have, when we have them,
and how we experience and express these emotions.” Emotion
expression or emotion display are terms that describe the
same phenomenon of how emotions are communicated, which is a
constituent part of emotion regulation besides the internal
regulation of emotion (e.g. down-regulating the intensity
and duration of anger).
The (appropriate) expression of emotions in HE
is discussed in various ways in the literature. In an
overview, Gates (2000) identified studies in which authors
have argued that a neutral teaching and learning environment
may be best for students’ learning in HE classrooms, which
suggests emotion suppression. However, the majority of
literature advocates for authenticity, which allows for
teachers’ emotion display in a controlled manner (e.g.
Cranton & Carusetta, 2004). An authentic display of
emotions fulfills a relevant function in establishing
genuine and caring relationships with students (Yuu, 2010;
see also Fischer & Manstead, 2010 discussing the social
functions of emotions in general), and is also significant
for maintaining teachers’ health (Zhang & Zhu, 2008).
Research also suggests that emotion suppression can have
adverse cognitive implications (Gross, 2010), which in turn
may also impact teaching quality while cognitive resources
and attention are focused on the process of emotion
suppression.
The research strand on “emotional labour”
discusses the requirement to mask emotions on the job
(Hochschild, 1983). Employees aiming at successful
fulfillment of work tasks are expected to follow particular
occupational emotion display rules, which usually means
masking (overly intense) negative feelings and acting in an
emotionally positive manner. If students in HE are regarded
as customers, this also applies to teachers as department
employees with teaching duties. According to Fischer,
Manstead, Evers, Timmers and Valk (2004), the display of
desired emotions in the job does not necessarily cause
negative side effects, presupposing that appropriate
regulation of emotions is accepted as a part of teachers’
role-identity. However, negative consequences may result if
emotional dissonance occurs, since the expressed emotions do
not coincide with one’s identity. If teachers experience
emotional labour over a sustained period, it could result in
negative consequences, such as decreased satisfaction in the
job, or burnout symptoms (e.g. emotional exhaustion)
(Barber, Grawitch, Carson, & Tsouloupas, 2010; Zhang
& Zhu, 2008). Emotional labour can also arise from the
role of a teacher, a job or function that includes moral
elements (Chen & Kristjansson, 2011), such as pastoral
care (Isenbarger & Zembylas, 2006; Oplatka, 2007; Yuu,
2010) or being role models. From this perspective, it is
pertinent that teachers as educators are in control of their
emotions, which requires a degree of emotional labour.
Concluding, lack of competence in appropriate
communication of emotion can not only damage the wellbeing
of HE teachers but also endangers the development of
positive relationships with students. Consequently, emotions
and the display of emotions contribute to the quality of
interactions (Boiger & Mesquita, 2012). At university,
interactions between teachers and students occur formally in
courses and informally on campus. These interactions
ultimately lead to the establishment of relationships, which
are multilayered as they are built on a professional level
(= working relationship) and on an interpersonal level (=
closeness, affiliation) (Hagenauer & Volet, 2014c; Nias,
1989). Richardson and Radloff (2014) have highlighted the
significance of teacher-student-interactions for HE
students’ positive experiences, however they also cautioned
that the frequency of direct interactions steadily decreases
due to changes in the HE context (e.g. increases in
student-staff ratio or online learning; as found in the
Australian context). This development is alarming, given the
fact, that fulfillment of the basic need for belongingness
or relatedness (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Deci &
Ryan, 2002) is likely to be relevant for both – HE teachers
(e.g. in terms of their workplace satisfaction) and HE
students (e.g. in terms of their study commitment).
As empirical evidence on the role of emotions
in teacher-student interaction in HE is rather scarce, we
draw from research on schoolteachers in order to better
frame this phenomenon. From this research we know that
interactions with students are frequently emotionally laden
and that student-teacher-interactions are the most prevalent
source of teacher emotions (for a review, see Sutton &
Wheatley, 2003). Recently, Hagenauer, Hascher and Volet
(2015) have shown that “closeness” (as a result of many
positive student-teacher interactions) to students (e.g.,
liking students; knowing them personally to some degree)
predicted teachers’ emotions experienced when teaching in
the classroom. The effect was particularly strong for the
experience of joy. More generally, the model on teacher
emotions developed by Frenzel (2014) suggests, that
teachers’ perceptions regarding the degree of goal
attainment in particular teaching and/or interaction
situations with single students, groups of students or
classrooms, determines the quality of the emotion evoked.
Frenzel’s model relies on an appraisal-theoretical approach
to emotions and shows overlaps with the control-value theory
of achievement emotions, which is primarily applied in
research on students’ emotions (Pekrun, 2006). According to
that model, a teacher might experience anger if students are
unengaged in class, on the expectation that participation is
linked to high achievement goals, performance and
motivation.
Furthermore, it is reasonable to assume that
teachers and students’ emotions are dynamically linked. Students are
expected to react sensitively to their teachers’ emotions,
and reciprocally teachers would typically notice the
emotional state of their students (Garner, 2010).
This was demonstrated in a study by Frenzel, Goetz, Luedtke,
Pekrun and Sutton (2009), which revealed that teacher’s joy
supported school-students’ enjoyment in learning. In the HE
context, Titsworth, McKenna, Mazer and Quinlan (2013)
detected a link between teacher immediacy, a quality
indicator of a positive nonverbal teacher-student
interaction, and positive student emotions. The phenomenon
of emotions that spread from person to person is termed
“emotional contagion” (e.g. Hatfield, Bensman, Thornton,
& Rapson, 2014).
Thus, the quality of teacher-student
interactions in the classroom is highly relevant for the
concrete emotional experiences of the actors involved.
However, empirical evidence on emotions and emotion display
in teacher-student interactions in HE, and more specifically
in teacher education, is lacking. With regard to teacher
education, some research has focused on the emotions of
student teacher in the teaching practicum (e.g., Hascher
& Hagenauer, 2016; Pillen, Beijaard & den Brok,
2013; Timostsuk & Ugaste, 2012) or school-based teachers
(Hastings, 2008); but the emotional aspects of the
university-based part of teacher education are overlooked.
1.2
Cultural aspects of emotion display and
relationships
Cross-cultural
comparisons in emotion research are not new (e.g. Eid &
Diener, 2001). According to Mesquita (2007), emotions are
“culturally situated” (p. 410) and are not just an
individual phenomenon. Emotions can be described as
socio-cultural phenomena since most human emotions are
evoked in social situations of interaction (Boiger &
Mesquita, 2012).
Many studies of emotions across cultures have
focused on the expression of emotions in social situations,
which are underpinned by so-called display rules. For
Safdar et al. (2009, p.1), display rules “influence the
emotional expression of people from any culture depending on
what that particular culture has characterized as an
acceptable of unacceptable expression of emotion”. Besides
personality traits that affect emotion display (e.g. people
with high scores on extraversion display emotions more
intensively; Matsumoto, 2006), culture has been claimed to
be an influencing factor on emotion display. In countries
categorized as “individualistic” (Hofstede & Hofstede,
2005), where a strong independent self is highly valued,
different modes of emotion display can be observed in
comparison to countries labeled “collectivistic”, where an
interdependent self is encouraged (Markus &
Kitayama, 1991). Despite evidence that the display of
(negative) emotions is regarded as the right of the
individual in “individualistic” countries, from a
collectivistic point of view, emotions tend to be controlled
in favor of the enhancement of positive relationships and
harmony (Safdar et al., 2009). Furthermore, in countries
labeled “individualistic” people have been found to value
high intensive positive emotions (e.g. excitement), whereas
people in more collectivistic cultures strive for the
experience of low(er) intensive positive emotions (e.g.
feeling calm) (Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, 2006). Based on
Hofstede and Hofstede’s research (2005), Western countries,
such as the U.S., Australia and many European countries, are
considered “individualistic” countries, and East Asian
countries as “collectivistic”. However, such a broad-brush
categorization has been widely criticized, given that there
is great variability within cultural dimensions, including
in terms of emotion display (Schwarz & Ros, 1995).
Consequently, there is a need for extending our
understanding of emotion display rules within, and not only
between, individualistic and collectivistic countries, which
has been largely unexplored to date (see also Koopman-Holm
& Matsumoto, 2011, comparing the U.S. with Germany).
Furthermore, in HE, teachers act in a
professional setting that has its own display rules. While
emotions must be more controlled in workplace settings in
comparison to private settings (Moran, Diefendorff, &
Greguras, 2013), another dimension requiring attention in
cross-cultural emotion display research is that of context,
and the roles people play and relationships they form in
particular contexts. Therefore in addition to potential
variability of emotion display within so called
“individualistic” and “collectivistic” countries, the
context and situation should be considered (see also Volet,
2001). An actor will vary in emotion display depending on
the current role, as mother/father or university lecturer
for example, and will also be impacted by the particular
relationship quality. As Safdar et al. (2009) observed, the
display mode of emotion is usually contingent on the
interaction partner, this interdependency having
consequences for the relationship, due to the reciprocal
influence of emotion display and relationship-quality
(Boiger & Mesquita, 2012; Eid & Diener, 2001;
Fischer & Manstead, 2010). Concluding,
similarly to the display of emotions, the quality of the
TSR is influenced by cultural and institutional
(organizational) norms, functioning as cultural guidance
modeling “ideas, meanings, and practices of how to be a
person and how to relate to others” (Boiger &
Mesquita, 2012, p. 224).
1.3
The present study: Aims and relevance
The present study
explored how Australian and German university teachers, in
so-called “individualistic” countries, displayed their
positive and negative emotions when teaching at university
and how they perceived a positive TSR. The review article of
Quinlan (2016) on HE teaching and learning underpins the
importance of relationships for emotions in interactions and
vice versa, which stresses the need for more empirical
research on that issue. Furthermore, this study aimed to
deepen understanding about university teachers’ emotion
display through a cross-cultural lens. This research is
timely given the pace of HE’s internationalization (Altbach
& Knight, 2007) in both research and teaching (e.g.,
Shimmi, 2014). Understanding the cultural specifics of both
home and host country appears imperative for cultural
adaptation, and the quality of teaching practice from an
international perspective. More specifically, the following
research questions were addressed:
a) What do German and
Australian teacher educators perceive as appropriate emotion
display in terms of positive and negative emotions when
teaching and interacting with their students? What are
similarities and differences in their perceptions?
b) How do German and
Australian teacher educators construe the quality of the
TSR, and the “ideal TSR”? What are similarities and
differences in their views?
c) How do German and
Australian teacher educators’ modes of emotion display and
views of the quality of the TSR quality interrelate?
d) How do particular
background variables (e.g. position at university,
background as a school teacher) contribute to explain the
mode of emotion display and views of the quality of the TSR
among Australian and German teacher educators?
The choice of
samples within the individualistic cluster was based on
convenience, and represent the authors’ respective, personal
cultural background and familiarity with specific
cultural-educational HE contexts. However, this
choice also addresses concerns that cross-cultural
comparisons should go beyond the frequently made broad
discrimination of collectivistic versus individualistic
countries. Indeed, the two individualistic countries chosen
for the present study score differently on Hofstede and
Hofstede’s (2005) degree of individualism. Based on their
“individualism index” (p. 78), Australia ranks higher on
individualism (2 out of 74 countries/regions) compared to
Germany (ranked 18). Consequently, it was reasonable to
anticipate some variability in display practices as well as
perceptions of quality TSR, which could be interpreted in
regard to their respective cultural-educational contexts
(cultural and educational dimensions being confounded).
2.
Method
2.1
The participants
Fifteen
Australian (6 male, 9 female) and nine German (5 male, 4
female) teacher educators from two public universities in
Australia and one public university in Germany participated
voluntarily in the study. Participants for the study were
approached informally by one of the authors from the same
cultural-educational context. Selection criteria aiming at
achieving representativeness in terms of relevant
demographic characteristics pertaining to the population of
teacher educators were: (1) at least two years of teaching
experience (as there is evidence that the emotional
experiences of beginning teachers are of particular quality,
e.g. Ria, Sève, Saury, Theureau, & Durand, 2003); (2)
teaching in different subject areas across teacher
education, for example, introductory courses in educational
psychology, school pedagogics, mathematics and science
education, civic education, and literacy education (as
different subject areas may attract different styles of
“communication”); (3) holding different positions in the
university, ranging from full professor, to (full-time;
part-time) lecturer, to PhD-student with teaching duties (as
particular positions in the university system are known to
determine HE teachers’ duties, and in turn the relative
importance they give to teaching, frequency of interactions
with students etc.). This purposive sampling strategy
captured the typical heterogeneity of the population of
teacher educators in these two countries, which is necessary
for exploring the phenomenon in its breadth.
The Australian
sample comprised twelve lecturers in teacher education
(e.g., associate lecturer, senior lecturer, lecturer; most
of them at post-doctoral level), and three Associate
Professors in Education with broader teaching and research
responsibilities. In contrast, the German sample consisted
of five full Professors, one full-time lecturer, one
post-Doctoral fellow, and two PhD students with teaching
duties. The cultural-educational background of participants
differed across countries The Australian sample was
culturally more diverse than the German sample, which is
typical of the profile of German and Australian teacher
educators in general. None of the German teacher educators
had any personal or professional experience of another
cultural-educational setting, whereas five Australian
teachers came from another country. However, these teachers
had already some years of teaching experience in Australia
before the interview was conducted. To protect the anonymity
of the Australian participants, no details pertaining to
their specific cultural background can be provided.
2.2
The context: Teacher education in
Germany and in Australia
Teacher
education in Germany is structured in two main phases. Phase
1 covers predominantly academic studies at a university (in
general for 6 to 10 semesters), including some phases of
school practice. However, most of the practical
preparation is provided in a second phase, taking place in
special, generally small, institutions operated by state
governments and known as Studienseminare. The second phase
typically lasts 24 months (König & Blömeke, 2013). The
present study took place in the first phase of teacher
education at a University in Thuringia. No selection
procedures (numerus
clausus) for entry into teacher education are applied
at that university. Thus, all students who had successfully
completed their Abitur,
the secondary school completion diploma, were qualified to
commence teacher education. Students at that university
studied teacher education to practice in high-track
secondary schools, so called Gymnasien or Realschulen.
Typically, most of these students have an academic family
background and are not from a low SES or migrant family
background.
Teacher education in Australia is structured
differently, depending on whether it prepares students to
become primary school teachers, or secondary school teachers
specializing in the teaching of particular subjects, e.g.
mathematics, languages, science. In the two
universities where the study was conducted, students taught
by the respondents were predominantly future primary school
teachers. Primary teacher education, for students coming
straight out of high school (the majority), is usually
completed in a four-year period. Academic subjects are
typically interspersed with practical preparation during the
first three years with a strong practicum component in the
last year. Entry into primary teacher education is based on
academic results in high school, or completion of other
tertiary study, but entry levels tend to be lower than in
other fields of university study. A substantial proportion
of future primary school teachers, taught by the respondents
at these two universities, would have been from a low SES,
and possibly migrant family background, although primary
school teachers in Australia are not a culturally diverse
professional group, which contrasts somehow with the overall
diverse population.
2.3
Interviews and procedure
The first author
conducted semi-structured face-to-face individual interviews
with the teachers in each country. The style of the
interviews was open, informal and conversational. University
ethics approvals and informed consent from participants were
obtained prior to participation. Most of the interviews were
conducted in the interviewee’s offices on campus, and a few
in the staff room, if the teachers preferred this more
informal context. Interview duration ranged from 35 to 75
minutes; most interviews were 45-55 minutes. Interviews were
digitally recorded and transcribed verbatim. In the
Australian context, the interviews were in English; in the
German context they were in German. A German speaking person
proficient in English translated the quotes used in the
present article. For crosschecking, an English speaking
person with sound German language skills did the same,
leading to English transcriptions of the German interview
accounts.
A
semi-structured interview guideline framed the basic three
themes that were addressed during the interviews: (1)
emotions when teaching; (2) emotion regulation (internal
regulation and emotion display) and (3) the TSR at
university. In this paper, we refer to the interview
accounts on theme 2 – focusing on the display of emotions –
and theme 3 – the TSR. Emotional and relationship issues can
be addressed directly or indirectly in the interviews. In
our study, and consistent with ethical standards in
qualitative research, the purpose of the research was
communicated to the participants right from the beginning
and participants were asked direct questions pertaining to
the main focus of the research. Thus, the participants were
fully aware that the interview would focus on emotions and
relationships with students in HE. In other words, there was
no artificially masking the central topics through indirect
questions. Furthermore, we expected these experienced
teacher educators would have reflected on their teaching and
would feel ready to talk about their emotions and
relationships with students. In terms of emotion display,
the leading question was “Do you show and express
your feelings while teaching and interacting with students
or do you also hide them sometimes?” followed by
various probes that included descriptions of concrete
interaction situations that were experienced by the teachers
in teaching situations, focusing on teaching in small-groups
settings, such as seminars or workshops (up to about 30
students), and teaching first-year students. The teachers
also talked about concrete emotions, e.g. joy or anger. If
these accounts included information about the display of
emotions, they were coded within the category “emotion
display” as well. Regarding the TSR, teachers were asked to
describe the “ideal”
TSR at university from their perspective. Again,
probes were used to elicit further elaboration on the TSR.
If descriptions of emotional interaction situations
contained details pertaining to the TSR, these accounts were
coded within the category of TSR as well.
2.4
Data analysis
The interview
material was coded based on a category system in orientation
to a deductive-inductive qualitative content analysis
(Mayring, 2000; Gläser-Zikuda & Mayring, 2003). The
analysis involved several steps.
Step 1: First, a
theoretically based category system was developed to code
the content of the interviews. Then, the transcripts were
read several times, and all text passages that could be
allocated to the broad category of “emotion display”
(subcategories: display of positive emotions versus display
of negative emotions) or “the ideal TSR” were electronically
coded using the MAXQDA software (“structuring content
analysis” according to Mayring, 2000). This step was done by
the first author only, as due to the direct question format
the extraction of these interview accounts was a very clear
coding process with little room for interpretation. The
three main codes – illustrated in Table 1 – were derived
deductively from the theory and the main research questions.
To ensure an objective and reliable coding procedure, coding
rules were formulated and anchor examples identified in the
interviews.
Table 1
The coding scheme
Code |
Number of accounts |
Code description and anchor
example |
Displaying
positive emotions |
AUS: 46 GER: 28 |
This code is used when teachers talk
about how to display positive emotions. Example: So, okay,
the positive ones are easy to handle. Just
join it, just share the fun. |
Displaying
negative emotions |
AUS: 80 GER: 60 |
This code is used when teachers talk
about how to display negative emotions. Example: Uhm,
probably. I am sure I do. I am a bit of an open
book. So, I think, you know, I don’t ... I don’t
hide my feelings or even though I try to ... As I’ve
said I am not gonna show that I am angry. |
The
quality of the TSR |
AUS: 69 GER: 24 |
This code is used for teachers’ answers
on the question pertaining to “ideal TSR”. No
distinction is made between the professional and
interpersonal TSR at this coding step, as statements
on both dimensions are frequently intertwined. Example: I am careful, because I don’t want
it to seem unprofessional. But yeah, I also think
it’s important for them to see that ... It’s a whole person. |
Step 2: After that, and
in order to get greater insight into each individual teacher
and his/her perception in terms of negative and positive
emotion display and the TSR, a summary for each interviewee
was prepared based on the extracted interview accounts
resulting from the first coding step (“summarizing content
analysis”; Mayring, 2000). These summaries provided a
concise overview of each case (or HE teacher educator).
After that, the original interview accounts and the
summaries formed the basis for coding each case according to
the relevant categories. A separate coding scheme for
negative emotion display, positive emotion display and the
TSR was applied to each case as the unit of analysis.
[1] In terms of the
display of negative emotions (= anger), three aspects were
coded:
a) Did the teacher perceive
the direct communication of anger as appropriate or not?
(1= yes; 2 = no; 3 = ambivalent)
b) Did the teacher think
that the communication of anger has to be controlled? (1 =
yes; 2 = no; 3 = ambivalent)
c) How does the teacher
communicate his/her anger to the students? (1 = I-messages;
2 = group-messages; 3 = transfer messages; 4 =
argumentative confrontation; 5 = provocative
confrontation; 6 = sarcasm, irony; 7 =
raising the voice; 8 = threat / classroom relegation; 9 =
one-on-one contact after the lesson; 10 = I-messages (using
less intensive words)
While the codes
1 and 2 were derived deductively, the codes applied in (3)
(= communication of anger) were inductively developed from
the data.
[2] In terms of the
display of positive emotions, two aspects were coded:
a) Did the teacher perceive
the direct communication of positive emotions as appropriate
or not? (1= yes; 2 = no; 3 = ambivalent). As not
many accounts revealed information on the control of
positive emotions, the code “emotion control” was not
applied for the display of positive emotions.
b) How does the teacher
communicate his/her positive emotions? (1= positive feedback
(neutral); 2 = praising students; 3 =
communicating positive emotions intensively (verbally); 4 =
hugging students; 5 = displaying enthusiasm; 6 = sharing
humour).
Again, code 1
was derived deductively from theory, and the concrete
emotional communication inductively from the data.
[3] In terms of the
ideal TSR two codes were applied:
a) Does the teacher perceive
the professional aspect of the TSR as relevant? (1
= yes, 2 = no, 3 = ambivalent)
b) Does the teacher perceive
the interpersonal aspect of the TSR as relevant? (1 = yes,
2 = no, 3 = ambivalent)
Both codes were
derived deductively according to the conceptualization of
the TSR (Hagenauer & Volet, 2014c). As an
example, the coding scheme for the display of positive
emotions is illustrated in Table 2.
Table
2
The coding scheme
for the display of positive emotions
Code |
Definition |
Anchor example |
Direct expression of positive
emotions |
|
|
Yes (1) |
“Yes” is coded, if teachers say that they
communicate positive emotions to students. |
|
No (2) |
“No” is coded, if teachers do not
communicate their positive emotions to students. |
|
Ambivalent (3) |
“Ambivalent” is coded if teachers basically agree
that showing positive emotions is possible, but are
hesitant about it at the same time. |
|
Way of communicating emotions |
|
|
Giving positive feedback (1) |
The teacher communicates positive
emotions neutrally by giving content-focused
feedback. |
I give feedback to the students at the
end of the session how I perceived the session. How
I perceived the progress of the course. (I5,
Germany) |
Praising students (2) |
The teacher communicates positive
emotions (e.g., satisfaction) by praising the
students. Praise incorporates some kind of
emotionality in the feedback. |
Well, from my perspective praising
students is very important. (I6, Germany) |
Expressing positive emotions
intensively (verbally) (3) |
The teacher communicates positive
emotions intensively verbally. The communication is
more intensively compared to code 2. |
I would equally say "I am so happy for
you." You know, if somebody gets a job or if
somebody gets an award or something else. (I4,
Australia) |
Hugging students (intense physical
reaction) (4) |
The teacher hugs the students. |
I would hug students and students would
hug me that ... that ... not all the time but I
wouldn't hold back from doing that kind of thing.
(I4, Australia) |
Displaying enthusiasm (5) |
The teacher shows enthusiasm evoked by
the content / subject. |
I get excited about things. And I'll
say: Oh, guess what, guys! Look at this! Check this
out! Everybody come over! (I13, Australia) |
Sharing humour (6) |
The teacher shares humour in the
classroom. |
Having a laugh with the group. That's
important. (I15, Australia) |
Interrater-reliability
was calculated for the second coding step. Interview
accounts from eight interviewees (four from Germany, four
from Australia), which represents about a third of the whole
data set, were randomly selected and coded independently by
two of the authors. Both are fluent in German and English,
which was critical as the German interviews were not
translated to English. The result of the double-coding
procedure is presented in Table 3. Percentage agreement was
92.60 % (54 coding options; 4 disagreements). In order to
account for randomly reached agreement, the corrected
Cohen’s kappa was calculated (Brenan & Prediger, 1981).
It lied at .85, which is satisfactory (Bortz & Döring,
2006). All disagreements were discussed between the two
coders (including also going back to the whole interview
incorporating any information that would help to clarify the
respective code) until agreement was reached. After that,
the first author went back to the data and validated the
codes of the other interviews taking into account the
aspects that had been discussed between the two coders.
Table 3
Interrater agreement
(selection of 8/24 teacher educators, 4 German and 4
Australian)
|
GER1 |
GER2 |
GER3 |
GER4 |
AUS1 |
AUS2 |
AUS3 |
AUS4 |
Display of positive emotions |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
Display of negative emotions |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
Interpersonal teacher-student
relationship |
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
Emotion control (display of negative
emotions) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
Communicating positive emotions (1) |
x |
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
Communicating positive emotions (2) |
x |
x |
|
|
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
Communicating positive emotions (3) |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Communicating negative emotions (1) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
|
|
x |
x |
Communicating negative emotions (2) |
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
Note.
x = agreement; (x) = disagreement; empty field = n.a., if
for example, an interviewee only mentioned one way of
communicating positive emotions.
Step 3: The final step
complemented the comparison of emotion display modes and the
quality of the TSR across the two countries based on the
interview accounts extracted in coding step 1 and the case
summaries resulting from coding step 2. Regularly revisiting
the original transcripts was undertaken at step 3 in order
to confirm the accuracy of inferred conclusions. Step 3
resulted in a case overview pertaining to the three main
categories, namely, “display of negative emotions”, “display
of positive emotions” and the “ideal TSR”.
3.
Findings
As
aforementioned in the introduction, emotion display and
relationships are reciprocally entwined. The findings are
structured around the four research questions, (1) starting
by German and Australian teacher educators’ perceptions of
appropriate emotion display in their interactions with
students, (2) followed by their views of the quality of the
TSR at university and the “ideal TSR”, (3) the relationship
between their reported modes of emotion display and views of
the quality of the TSR, (4) and the examination of
background variables that may contribute to explain modes of
emotion display and views of the quality of the TSR across
samples.
3.1
Modes of emotion display (Res Q 1)
3.1.1 Modes
of positive emotion display
Across
interviews, teacher educators stressed issues concerning the
display of positive emotions less frequently than the
display of negative emotions. For most, it was clear that
positive emotions evoked in the classroom are shared easily,
although the majority of German teacher educators appeared
more reluctant in expressing positive emotions in an
(emotionally) intense and direct manner than their
Australian counterparts. Australian teacher educators
frequently expressed strong positive feelings about
students, as illustrated by emotionally laden expressions,
such as “being thrilled about” or “getting very excited in
the classroom”. In contrast, German teacher educators
reported expressing positive emotions less directly, mainly
communicating their satisfaction or joy by praising the
achievement of particular students or a group. When probed
about displaying positive feelings in the classroom, German
teacher educators mostly tended to respond in a way similar
to this example:
I:
“And how about the positive emotions? Do you express them
as well?”
A:
„Yes, it is very similar. At the end of each
course I give feedback how I experienced it. (I5,
male, Germany)
Thus,
in the German context positive emotions were typically
reflected in teacher’s feedback or praise, and commonly
communicated in a relatively neutral manner. Alternatively,
the Australian teachers reported more emotion-laden
interactions in expressing praise. For example,
So
I have sent an announcement to everyone saying, I am
really thrilled and proud of the feedback you are giving
on your (anonymized; virtual platform). (I8, male,
Australia)
A
female Australian teacher also mentioned that she would hug
students if she felt deep joy, e.g. due to the success of a
particular student. Further, some Australian accounts
revealed how emotions transfer between teachers and students
(e.g. a student feels happy following success; this
happiness transfers to the teacher).
Taken
together, it is clear that expressing positive emotions was
widely regarded as appropriate and relevant (e.g., in terms
of fostering students motivation) in both
cultural-educational contexts. However, the actual mode of
expression differed somewhat across contexts. This pertained
mainly to the student-teacher interaction (e.g. how to
praise students) and less to the teacher-subject-interaction
(e.g., how to respond emotionally on subject matter).
Australian and German teacher educators expressed relaying
enthusiasm for their taught subject similarly, but a little
more pronounced in the Australian context, as illustrated by
the following quote:
I
mean, I get excited about things. Very enthusiastic about
the science. And I’ll say: Oh guess what, guys! Look at
this! Check this out! Everybody come over! (imitates
excitement in a classroom). Now, is that emotional? Yes! I
get very enthused about the science or you know, that type
of thing. It’s really cool stuff. But as far as reacting
to students, I try not to be way up or way down. (I13,
female, Australia)
Finally,
what was perceived as appropriate display of emotions varied
among teachers from the same country, indicating that the
expression of emotions may be influenced not only by the
cultural-educational context but also by individual
characteristics, such as personality or position at
university. For example, contrary to most of her colleagues,
one German teacher reported teaching in a very emotional
manner, as she had experienced that kind of enthusiasm as a
relevant antecedent of learner’s motivation during her
former work as a schoolteacher.
3.1.2 Modes
of negative emotion display
In
terms of negative emotions, annoyance dominated in both the
German and Australian accounts. Therefore the following
analysis focused on teachers’ annoyance and anger (for an
overview on the range of negative and positive emotions
typically expressed by teacher educators, see Hagenauer
& Volet, 2014a).
In the present
study, we focused on the most frequently mentioned negative
behaviours that evoked teachers’ anger or annoyance (e.g.
student disengagement or classroom disturbance). Comparing
case summaries revealed a marked difference in the accounts
of the German and Australian teachers. While nearly all the
Australian teachers shared the opinion that negative
emotions should be suppressed for professional and
role-modeling reasons, German teachers were less reluctant
in expressing their anger directly to students. But both
groups shared the belief that the display of anger should be
controlled according to professional standards. More
concretely, most Australian teacher educators advocated for
the need to suppress negative feelings in class. If
classroom disturbances occurred, both groups claimed that
the teacher should intervene calmly if the disturbance
affected the learning of the others but would strategically
ignore the disturbance if the classroom learning process
were not endangered. They usually would not express negative
emotions directly, but would talk to the particular
student(s) on a one-on-one basis in or after the course to
address the problematic behavior:
But if they talk to each other, I would
mention that. Yeah, I would say: I would expect you, you
know, you are listening at this point. I try and do that
sometimes in a private way rather than publicly. (I14,
male, Australia)
In contrast, the
German teacher educators reported a variety of reactions
when they experienced anger, which were made visible to
students more pronounced relative to their Australian
counterparts. For example, one German female teacher
reported that she experienced anger if students submitted
assignments the evening before the seminar. Upon probing how
she expressed her anger, she replied:
I tell them, it is not ok, if they don’t get
the timing right and I have to pay for it. It’s not
acceptable for me. I don’t want that and then I also
justify why I don’t want it that way. (I9,
female, Germany)
Only one of the
nine German teacher educators said he suppressed his anger
completely, which he traced back to his personality and his
difficulty in coping with conflict. The other German teacher
educators reported using mainly verbal strategies in such
circumstances, such as sending I-messages addressing
problematic behaviour and how it affected them (“I am annoyed
that…”), or the group (“Do you think it is fair to your
group members…?”), or asking the students how they would
cope with such situations in their own classroom (bringing
in the professional perspective). I-messages were also a
popular method to deal with disturbances from the Australian
teachers’ perspective. If perceived as necessary, problems
were also addressed verbally; however experienced negative
emotions were frequently addressed more cautiously, by
substituting with a less-intense emotion display. For
example, one teacher reported dealing with anger and
disappointment in class contributions:
I mean, if you’re disappointed, if you are
not happy with something, you have got to tell them. And
I’ll tell them. If I say: Look, I am not, I am not
pleased. Nobody seems to be contributing to this
discussion today. We need to contribute to discussions.
This is how we learn. We need to talk about things. So,
you know, please get involved.” (I9, male, Australia)
When
students expressed opposing views, or displayed lack of
openness, which caused anger in some of the German teachers’
accounts, the German teachers frequently dealt with their
annoyance through starting an argumentative and sometimes
provocative discussion in class. Some teachers also reported
raising their voice or using sarcasm as verbal reactions in
situations they found annoying. One teacher mentioned that
he was willing to react directly by removing the problematic
student from the classroom if the student did not respond
favorably after a few prompts.
Some reactions
to anger that were mentioned by the German teacher educators
would be perceived as problematic in the Australian context.
For example, the display of anger was interpreted as a means
of maintaining a productive professional working
relationship from some German teachers’ perspective, since
not displaying anger, and not intervening in difficult
classroom situations would be regarded in that context as
displaying a lack of professionalism. It should be noted,
however, that such views were mainly expressed by less
experienced teachers. In contrast, one Australian teacher
stated that he “would lose face” if he showed his annoyance
directly in class. Another Australian teacher mentioned that
he did not want to risk the progress of the work by letting
negative emotions interfere while another female Australian
teacher said she would not want to risk the positive
relationships with students by reacting angrily.
Figure 1 and 2
and Table 4 outline the findings pertaining to the overall
display and communication of positive and negative emotions.
Each teacher educator is listed as a case in the table and
is represented within one column. In the Figures the
percentage of cases within each group of teacher educators
(German, Australian) is provided. The calculation of the
percentage made it possible to compare the responses of
German and Australian teacher educators directly as within
each group – although based on a different frequency - they
finally summed up to 100 %. Thus, in the Australian sample a
teacher educator accounts for about 7 %; in the German
sample for about 11 %.
As can be seen,
German and Australian teacher educators shared many ways of
displaying their emotions in the classroom, but there were
also noticeable differences. German teacher educators
frequently reported direct display of anger but this was not
the case among the Australians. In contrast, the direct
display of positive emotions was slightly more pronounced
within the Australian compared to the German sample. The
German teacher educators also reported higher intensity of
direct anger display and also a higher variety of possible
responses. In particular, German teacher educators reported
using various forms of verbal reactions when facing
difficult student behaviour, while their Australian
counterparts tended to avoid direct confrontation and
preferred talking to their students after class one-on-one.
Verbal and rational reactions, termed as “positive
feedback”, also seemed to be the German teachers’ preferred
way of displaying positive emotions, while the emotional
aspect of feedback/praise came through emotionally more
intensely in the Australian teachers’ accounts.
Figure 1: Ways of
communicating positive emotion by German and Australian
teacher educators (see pdf)
Figure 2: Ways of
communicating anger by German and Australian teacher
educators (see pdf)
Table
4
Display of positive
and negative emotions: Case overview comparing German and
Australian teacher educators’ perceptions
German sample |
|
Australian sample |
|
||||||||||||||||||||||
G1 |
G2 |
G3 |
G4 |
G5 |
G6 |
G7 |
G8 |
G9 |
A1 |
A2 |
A3 |
A4 |
A5 |
A6 |
A7 |
A8 |
A9 |
A10 |
A11 |
A12 |
A13 |
A14 |
A15 |
|
|
f |
f |
m |
m |
m |
m |
f |
m |
f |
|
m* |
f* |
f* |
f* |
f |
f |
m |
f |
m |
f |
f |
m |
f* |
m |
m |
Gender |
Is it appropriate to display positive
emotions and anger (as a frequent negative
emotion) in HE teaching? (x = yes; (x) = ambivalent; empty
field = no) |
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
(x) |
(x) |
|
(x) |
x |
x |
x |
n.a. |
n.a. |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
Pos.E. |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
x |
|
x |
|
|
n.a. |
|
x |
|
x |
x |
|
|
(x) |
|
|
Anger |
|
Is it necessary to control the
display of negative emotions? (x =
yes; (x) = ambivalent) |
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
x |
x |
x |
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
x |
x |
n.a. |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
control |
The display of positive emotions:
reactions (x = mentioned in the interview) |
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
x |
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
POF |
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
PRA |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
IER/H |
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
x |
ENT |
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
(x) |
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
x |
|
x |
HUM |
|
The display of negative emotions:
reactions (x = mentioned in the interview) |
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
OoO |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
x |
x |
|
|
|
x |
x |
LIE |
|
|
x |
|
x |
x |
|
x |
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
I-M |
|
x |
|
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
G-M |
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
T-M |
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
AC |
|
x |
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
PC |
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
SAR |
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
RAI |
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
THR |
Note. Pos.E. = positive emotions; POF = positive
feedback; PRA = praise; IER/H = intensive emotional reaction
(verbally) + hugging students; ENT = enthusiasm; HUM =
humour; OoO = one-on-one contact after the lesson;
LIE = using less intensive emotion words; I-M = I-messages;
G-M = group messages; T-M = transfer messages; AC =
argumentative confrontation; PC = provocative
argumentation; SAR = sarcasm/irony; RAI = raising the voice;
THR = threat, classroom relegation; n.a. = not applicable
(no information on that aspect in the interview): *coming
from another cultural background;
3.2
Quality of the teacher-student
relationship (Res Q 2)
According to Hagenauer and Volet
(2014c) the quality of the TSR can be described in terms of
support (or professional) dimension or in terms of affective
(or interpersonal) dimension. Our findings are presented in
relation to these two dimensions, starting with the teachers’
views on the quality of the professional TSR.
3.2.1 The
professional teacher-student relationship
By
comparing the accounts and reflections of the Australian and
German teacher educators, it is evident that both groups
regarded the TSR as predominantly a professional one, with
particular boundaries that must not be overstepped, but with
“room to personalize” this relationship, as stated by one
Australian female teacher educator (I5). However, there were
also differences between the two groups of teachers,
particularly with regard to the amount of formality versus
informality of the interactions, and the amount of
(interpersonal) care expressed within this relationship.
Most
of the German teacher educators described their role within
the professional TSR mainly as being a moderator, a
generator of ideas and inspiration or a specialist in terms
of the teaching content, who designs effective learning
environments in which students are required to contribute
actively. This professional TSR understanding is illustrated
below:
I would like to be an instigator. I want to
challenge them, they should think about stuff, care about
things, which I think are relevant, yes, be an
inspiration, a person who challenges, you know…the person,
who asks good questions and starts new thinking processes.
I’m not a Guru. I can see that with some colleagues and that’s scary to me, you know,
to have something like a fan club. I’m not the
head-teacher. I’m more the person who asks questions. An
instigator. (I9, female, Germany)
In
this professional working-relationship mutual appreciation
and respect are important components from the teacher
educator’s perspective and a well-adjusted give-and-take
basis is expected (e.g., in terms of engagement). A male
professor called this kind of relationship “mutual receptiveness
and openness” (I3).
Furthermore, many German teacher educators also expressed
approachability as a relevant dimension of the relationship,
equalizing approachability mostly with openness for
content-related questions of students. Informal contact
between students and teachers apart from the regular course
setting and the official office-hours was rare. The
following quote gives an example of a male teacher’s
perception on approachability, which also addresses the idea
that a certain distance in the TSR might not only be a need
of teachers but also of students:
The direct connection with students, if they
[the students] want it, it’s not a problem for me. In
lectures I say to them: if you have questions or if you
need anything else you can come and talk to me during
office hours, or they can have an additional appointment.
It’s all possible. If I have the time, I will give it to
them. But it has to be in a, you know, professional
setting. Well, it needs to stay connected to the topic.
(I5, male, Germany)
In
another interview a female university teacher-professor
stressed that office hours must be kept, the dilemma being
that good teaching is frequently unrewarded, which affects
the amount of effort invested into teaching. Ultimately,
this also impacts on the frequency and intensity of
teacher-student interactions:
I plan 1.5 hours for the office hour, most of
the time I need 2, I use a watch for it. Well, they would
like to be looked after for half an hour. That’s not
possible. After 15 minutes they have to leave …at the
latest. They have to ask precisely. It’s strange, but it
feels that university teaching stops me from my work. […]
Umm, and it’s not that I don’t think that teaching is not
important, it just feels, like doing something that nobody
sees or doesn’t count. That’s the frustrating thing. (I9,
female, Germany)
3.2.2
The
interpersonal teacher-student relationship
Australian
teacher educators emphasized mutual respect and appreciation
as relevant characteristics of the TSR, but they considered
its quality in terms of informality in interactions. Thus,
within the interpersonal dimension of the TSR some sort of
“closeness” is coming into play, as the following account
reveals:
I think closeness... and caring is quite
important. We routinely here, as you noticed, we are not
status-bound. I introduce myself to my students as XY
(first name). I say: Just call me XY (first name). And I
want them to see me as someone who is here to help them,
not someone who has an authority-status.[…] So, we have an
ethos in tune. I think we've always had that. (I7,
male, Australia)
In
contrast, “closeness” was viewed skeptically by most of the
German teacher educators:
I would say, relationships between students
and teachers or lecturers should always be at a
professional level. Of course there are sympathies. There
are aversions. They are okay and legitimate. But they
musnt’t disturb the sequence [of the course]. (I5, male,
Germany)
As expected, these “relational”
differences underpinned teacher-student interactions. In
contrast to Germany, it was found that Australian students
could call their university teachers by their first name, and
many teachers reported an open-door policy, so students could
approach them whenever they wanted to. It appeared also not
unusual in the Australian context for teachers and students to
share personal information, sometimes during the course but
also anywhere on campus. A male teacher discussed his
willingness in establishing interpersonal relationships:
I try and take an interest in them personally
...with their jobs and their families and the rest of
their lives. And I try and make it clear that I am not
just a teacher of [subject area]. I do other things as
well. (I7, male, Australia)
Furthermore,
for the Australian teachers, approachability implied that
students could approach them when they had content-related
questions, but also if they were dealing with personal
issues that interfered with their study. Aspects of personal
care that appeared to come into play were more visible in
the interview accounts of the Australian teacher educators.
This might be partly related to the fact that many students
in the two Australian universities in the present study were
from a lower socioeconomic background. During the
interviews, a number of teacher educators expressed worry or
concern about the study success of these students and
expressed a willingness to listen to their problems, for
example, by granting extensions in terms of submitting
assignments and spending extra time one-on-one to discuss
open questions.
And they have ...you know, there are lots of
personal issues, particularly in a lower socioeconomic
area ...[…] I generally find, that if someone has an issue
and if you manage to build that rapport and that
relationship they are happy to come and talk to me about
it and to say, you know, that they're struggling because
of being at the doctor last week and having heart tests
and that they're, you know, are so stressed and not
knowing what the results are and things like that...that,
you know, I am happy to sort of say to them: Well, fine.
This assignment is due in Friday night. Monday is fine.
(I8, female, Australia)
Interestingly,
when probed for worry or concern, many of the German teacher
educators said that these emotions did not play any
important role in their teaching practice.
But in a workshop or seminar, whether they
achieve or not, I’m not too worried, because ultimately,
they are adults and I can’t do everything for them. (I2,
female, Germany)
Not only the
background of students, but also the system more generally
appeared to contribute to the explicit caring attitude of
teacher educators in Australia. As retention rate and
learning outcomes of students are considered by funding
bodies as important indicators of the “efficacy” of
university teaching, these teachers felt somehow obligated
to maximize students’ academic success, which sometimes
created friction:
It is hard, yeah. In Australia very much the
emphasis is to try and help them to pass. And that's
something I am not used to…because sometimes I think some
students really need to fail. (laughs). But you try and
help them as best as you can. So, if they are struggling
in language you'll offer them support in their language.
But sometimes you think: Really, this person shouldn't be
teaching! (laughs) Well I just, I sense that we are maybe
a little bit softer than other countries. (I12, male,
Australia)
Based on the
informality of the TSR and the amount of care invested in
this relationship, the interpersonal aspect of the TSR
emerged not only as stronger in the Australian sample, but
often as an explicit goal in teaching (e.g. building a
rapport with students). The informality and caring attitude,
highlighted by Australian teacher educators, was also
occasionally questioned, but mainly by teachers from another
cultural background, suggesting that such informality may
have to be learned by new teachers:
Here are more informal kinds of
relationships. But it doesn't mean...being informal does
not mean that there is no distinction between workshop
leader and students. I find it very hard to balance. It's
sometimes informal. Okay, we are like equal, you know. We
are like friends. […] So I adapted to it and, yeah, I
still need to, I am still learning. I feel, because it's a
long drawn thing having to find a nice balance or
effective balance. I don't have to be nice but I need to
be effective. (I1, male, Australia)
But not only the
informality constitutes an aspect that new teacher educators
may have to adapt to in the Australian context. The same may
apply to the adoption of an explicit “caring attitude” as
observed by non-native teacher educator, who described her
local colleagues as follows:
Most of the lecturers here would have been
teachers at some stage. So, most of them would come with
that caring attitude. You know, wanting to establish good
relationships, wanting to have, you know, like the best
possible environment, where they can teach and their
students can learn. (I2, female, Australia)
Taken together,
these findings show that Australian and German teacher
educators share a similar belief about the necessity to form
professional relationships with students at university.
However, due to differences in how interactions are realized
– in particular due to higher informality and more
pronounced caring attitude in Australia – the interpersonal
TSR seemed to be closer in the Australian sample, while the
professional and more formal working relationship dominated
in nearly all of the accounts of the German teachers (see
Table 5).
Table 5
Case overview: The
ideal teacher-student relationship (TSR): professional
and/or interpersonal?
|
German sample |
Australian sample |
||||||||||||||||||||||
|
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
1* |
2* |
3* |
4* |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
10 |
11 |
12 |
13* |
14 |
15 |
Gender |
f |
f |
m |
m |
m |
m |
f |
m |
f |
m |
f |
f |
f |
f |
f |
m |
f |
m |
f |
f |
m |
f |
m |
m |
Prof. TSR |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
Interp. TSR |
|
|
|
|
|
|
(x) |
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
x |
|
|
x |
x |
(x) |
x |
|
*coming from another cultural background
3.3
Relationship between modes of emotion
display, quality of the teacher-student relationship and
cultural-educational background of teacher educators (Res Q
3)
In a final step,
three target factors were interrelated: (1) mode of emotion
display; (2) quality of TSR and (3) cultural-educational
background of teacher educators (see Table 6). In order to
explain the relevance of the TSR on the display reactions of
emotion, the differences in display modes within the group
of German teacher educators and within the group of
Australian teacher educators was of interest (= controlling
for cultural-educational background). Looking at the
Australian sample, it became clear that the teacher
educators, whose TSR was more pronounced on an interpersonal
level, displayed their positive emotions with higher
intensity (marked in bold font, see Table 6); this was the
same for the German teacher educator who also formed strong
interpersonal relations with her students. Thus, for both
cultural-educational groups the intense communication of
positive emotions was strongly connected to the
interpersonal TSR. In regard to the communication of anger,
the Australian teacher educators communicated anger
similarly, regardless of whether they had formed a strong
interpersonal TSR or not. However, a slight difference
appeared: Two of the teacher educators, who formed the TSR
also on an interpersonal level, report using “moral”
strategies when communicating their anger (we-messages;
transfer messages). None of the Australian colleagues - who
focused less on the interpersonal TSR - reported these
strategies. Having a closer look at the German teacher
educator with a more pronounced affective bond to her
students, it became apparent that she used less-intensive
anger reactions than some of her colleagues who adopted a
greater distance and a less explicit caring attitude to
their students.
In a second step, a comparison within the
respective TSR group was made, in order to explore whether
cultural-educational differences were still noticeable when
accounting for the quality of the TSR. Within the group of
teachers who formed strong interpersonal TSR, the German
teacher educator showed similar reactions in terms of
communicating positive emotions; but her display of negative
emotions through I-messages was more directly compared to
the main reactions of the Australian counterparts, who
preferred mainly one-on-one contacts after the
course/workshop. Within the group of teachers who formed the
TSR mainly on a professional level, the obvious difference
in the direct communication of anger and positive emotions
between Australian and German teacher educators persisted:
On a professional level, the German teacher educators
communicated their positive emotions more neutrally, and
their anger more directly than their Australian
counterparts.
Thus, the results highlight two main points:
First, an emphasis on the interpersonal TSR mainly goes
along with the display of high intense positive emotions and
less intense negative emotions. Second, differences in
emotion display between Australian and German teacher
educators were found also within the respective TSR grouping
(professional + interpersonal, professional only), which
points to the importance of cultural-educational background
as an influencing factor on teacher educators’ emotion
display that goes above and beyond the quality of the TSR.
Table
6
Case overview: Display of positive and negative emotions and quality of the TSR, taking teacher educators’ cultural-educational background into account
Professional Relationship +
Interpersonal Relationship |
|
Professional Relationship only |
||||||||||||||||||||||
G7 |
A3 |
A4 |
A6 |
A7 |
A8 |
A11 |
A12 |
A13 |
A14 |
G1 |
G2 |
G3 |
G4 |
G5 |
G6 |
G8 |
G9 |
A1 |
A2 |
A9 |
A10 |
A15 |
|
|
f |
f* |
f* |
f |
m |
f |
f |
m |
f* |
m |
|
f |
f |
m |
m |
m |
m |
m |
f |
m* |
f* |
m |
f |
m |
|
Is
it appropriate to display positive emotions and
anger (as a frequent negative emotion) in HE
teaching? (x = yes; (x) = ambivalent; empty field
= no) |
||||||||||||||||||||||||
x |
x |
x |
n.a. |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
|
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
(x) |
(x) |
(x) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
Pos.E. |
x |
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
(x) |
|
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
x |
|
Anger |
|
Is it necessary to control the
display of negative emotions? (x =
yes; (x) = ambivalent) |
||||||||||||||||||||||||
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
x |
x |
(x) |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
x |
control |
The display of positive emotions:
reactions (x = mentioned in the interview) |
||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
x |
|
x |
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
POF |
x |
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
PRA |
|
x |
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
IER/H |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||
x |
|
|
|
x |
x |
x |
|
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
ENT |
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
x |
HUM |
The display of negative emotions:
reactions (x = mentioned in the interview) |
||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
|
x |
|
|
|
x |
x |
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
OoO |
|
|
x |
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
x |
x |
LIE |
|
x |
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
x |
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
I-M |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
G-M |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
T-M |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
AC |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
x |
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
PC |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
SAR |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
RAI |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
x |
x |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
THR |
Note. Light shading = German teacher educators; Dark
shading =Australian teacher educators; Pos.E. = Positive
Emotions; POF = positive Feedback; PRA = praise; IER/H =
intensive emotional reaction (verbally) + hugging students;
ENT = enthusiasm; HUM = humour; OoO = one-on-one contact
after the lesson; LIE = using less intensive emotion words;
I-M = I-messages;
G-M = group messages; T-M = transfer
messages; AC = argumentative confrontation; PC = provocative
argumentation; SAR = sarcasm/irony; RAI = raising the voice;
THR = threat, classroom relegation; n.a. = not applicable
(no information on that aspect in the interview); *coming
from another cultural background
A5 is not presented in the overview as
she explicitly stated that she “does not take the emotional
aspects” which made it impossible to code the main
categories on emotion display.
3.4
Factors contributing to explain modes of
emotion display and the quality of the teacher-student
relationship in German and Australian teacher educators:
Intensive and deviant case analyses (Res Q 4)
Finally, it was of
interest to explore the extent to which some central
background variables of these teacher educators may contribute
to explain the way they communicated their emotions in the
classroom and their views of the quality of the TSR. This
exploration was done through the close analysis of deviant and
intensive cases.
In the German
sample, the deviant case was G7, the teacher who reported
strong interpersonal relationships with her students, whereas
all other German teacher educators were allocated to the group
“professional TSR only”. Two German teachers (G2 and G5) who
expressed rather extreme negative emotions in comparison to
their colleagues were potential candidates for the intensive
case for the German context. In order to control for possible
gender effects, the female teacher (G2) was chosen for
comparison. In the Australian sample the deviant case was A15,
the teacher who expressed the most distant relationship to
students and who emphasized “professionalism” the most, and
the intensive case was A4, the teacher who showed the closest
relationship to students and adopted the most intensive way of
communicating her positive emotions. As background criteria
the following aspects were used: position at university, age,
experience in HE teaching, experience in school teaching,
amount of out-of-class interactions, dedication for HE
teaching (in teacher education) and having research duties
beyond teaching duties.
The results of the
comparisons are depicted in Figures 3 and 4. For both samples,
the identification that a teacher reveals from teaching in HE
- and in teacher education in particular - generated the most
salient difference in how interactions with students were
implemented (= dedication HE teaching). Both teachers who
described an intense display of positive emotions and
“close(r)” interpersonal relationships to students reported
that they loved teaching and were very keen on teaching in
teacher education, and enjoying the sharing of their own
experiences as school teachers to student teachers. G7 could
dedicate a lot of her time doing this, as she was a full-time
lecturer; thus the position matched her interests as the
following account illustrates:
That's a big part of my professional
understanding, and afterwards I go out of these classes and
I’m really ... feel like being on cloud nine. Then, I feel
really good, then I know: Yeah, that's my job, that’s what I
wanna do. You know when I can inspire them for what they
aspire to be. Wow, this really just warms my heart. So yes,
that's, um, that's really my idealism that I bring into this
job. You know, I really really like speaking about teaching,
I like teaching a lot, but I also like to talk about school
lessons, and I like to pique the students’ interest. (I7,
Germany
)
The dedication to
teaching in teacher education was not as visible in the
accounts of those two teachers who displayed more distant
relationships and a less intense way of communicating positive
emotions to their students. That might be traced back to
several factors: this German teacher did not have a school
teaching background herself and was quite inexperienced in HE
teachings, which was accompanied by some problems in classroom
management and thus feelings of insecurity. In the Australian
sample, position at university might have contributed to
differences. I15 had a background as a schoolteacher, but he
was a sessional lecturer (= an external lecturer) who taught
at university only once a week. He taught different courses on
that day and thus, the amount of informal interactions he
could have with students was limited. In addition, he
expressed that he was not sure how long he would teach at
university which suggested low(er) identification as a teacher
educator at university.
Figure
3: Case comparison: German sample (see pdf)
Figure
4. Case comparison: Australian sample (see
pdf)
4.
Discussion
The present study
explored the emotion display and the quality of the TSR at
university from a teachers’ perspective, with particular
attention to cultural-educational differences between a sample
of teachers from Australia and Germany. The findings revealed
many similarities but also differences between the Australian
and German teacher educators, in their emotion display and in
the quality of the TSR. Bringing these two strands of findings
together, and drawing on the literature on emotions in social
relations (Boiger & Mesquita, 2012; Parkinson et al.,
2005), it can be concluded that not only cultural aspects but
also the quality of the relationship formed between teachers
and students within their respective cultural HE setting, is
likely to impact on what is perceived by university teachers
as appropriate ways of displaying emotions. Although a
predominantly egalitarian TSR in the professional dimension
prevailed in both groups, in which considering the instructor
as facilitator supporting critical thinking and student
centered learning processes (in contrast to a predominantly
hierarchical TSR, which may be more common in the Chinese HE;
see Zhang & Zhang, 2013), the quality of the interpersonal
dimension of the TSR appeared to differ between the two
cultural-educational contexts.
Interpersonal
relations between teachers and students were relatively
pronounced in the Australian setting, while in the German
sample the professional working relationship between students
and teachers was emphasized, resulting in higher formality in
student-teacher interactions. The importance given to caring
in the interpersonal TSR by the Australian teacher educators might be partly explained in relation to the
higher proportion of their students being from low SES in
comparison to the German context. However, the formality
versus informality in the interactions that also substantially
contributed to the interpersonal TSR may be culturally
informed and less dependent on student characteristics. Future
studies will need to account for possible moderator variables,
such as the students’ background, when investigating
teacher-student interactions and relationships. As argued by
Safdar et al., (2009), relationship quality typically affects
the way emotions are communicated, as the “closeness” of the
interaction partner is regarded as a significant influencing
factor of emotion display. Typically, people express emotions
with higher intensity when relationships are close (Fischer
& Manstead, 2010). This was also evident in our study, but
only for the positive emotions: Teacher educators, who formed
more pronounced interpersonal relationships with their
students, displayed their positive emotions more intensely,
while they communicated the negative ones in a relatively
reserved way. On the other hand, if teachers formed
relationships mainly on a professional level, their positive
emotions were communicated less intensely.
Although all
teacher educators in this study believed in the necessity to
control emotions in professional settings, the way emotion
display was acted out in practice differed in the two
cultural-educational contexts. While the majority of the
Australian teachers communicated positive emotions immediately
but avoided the direct and immediate display of anger, their
German counterparts were less direct in the display of
positive emotions but more immediate in the communication of
anger. Furthermore, most of the German teacher educators did
not perceive the controlled expression of anger as a threat
for the professional TSR but instead regarded it as a
necessary component of their function as a teacher. Some
German teacher educators explicitly stated that it is not
about “liking them”, but about learning as much as possible
from their courses. These views are consistent with Yuu
(2010), who argued that expression of negative emotions in an
appropriate way does not risk relationships but bridges “the
psychological and emotional distance between teachers and
students” (p. 76). In contrast, some Australian teacher
educators stressed their effort not to display strong negative
emotions in order to maintain positive relationships with
students and not to risk authority. In a recent study, Bartram
(2015) found that sometimes students take advantage of close
and caring relationships with HE teachers, a behavior he
coined “affective strategizing” (p. 68). This suggests that a
caring attitude of HE teachers may not necessarily be
positive, as it comes with individual costs for the teacher
(e.g. a higher workload) and with students capitalizing on
this attitude (for a conceptualization on the caring HE
teacher, see Walker & Gleaves, 2016).
Interestingly, the
less pronounced acceptance of anger expression in the
Australian sample appears consistent with countries that score
higher on Hofstede and Hofstede’s (2005) collectivistic
dimension, whereas the display of negative emotions is
regarded as the right of the individual in more
individualistic countries. However, the conclusion that
Australian HE teacher educators might display more
collectivistic tendencies than their German counterparts
cannot be made based on the present data. In fact, Australia
scores higher on Hofstede and Hofstede’s individualism index
than Germany. In order to clarify that to some degree
unexpected finding, future research will need to explore the
extent to which different degrees of individualistic and
collectivistic tendencies can be found within so-called
“individualistic or collectivist” cultures / countries and how
these differences are connected to teacher motivation, emotion
and behaviour. We would argue that “individualism-index”
dimensions are not sensitive enough to interpret differences
in interactions and relationships, and that the educational
context and the norms, values and rules within this context
provide more promising explanatory elements. The combination
of cultural and contextual elements was captured in the term
“cultural-educational context” (Volet, 2001) that we have used
throughout the article.
Apart from
differences emerging between the accounts of the Australian
and German teacher educators, it is important to also note
their shared beliefs. For example, beliefs regarding the
necessity to control the display of negative emotions by
simultaneously maintaining a relatively high degree of
authenticity (an explicit goal in individualistic countries,
Safdar et al., 2009), were shared by Australian and German
teacher educators alike, which supports previous findings. For
example, Mendzheritskaya and Hansen (2013) (see also
Mendzheritskaya, Hansen & Horz, 2015) found that German
and Russian HE lecturers showed a greater authenticity in
displaying positive emotions, which was also the case in the
present sample, while negative emotions were displayed with
lower expressivity based on the belief that negative emotions
must be controlled in a professional setting. Another aspect
shared by many teachers in both countries, in particular in
Australia, was the importance of displaying enthusiasm for the
teaching subject in class. This is reminiscent of what Neumann
(2006) referred to as “passionate thought” inspired by
scholarly work in the respective subject area, and which
should, according to Neumann, be transferred to students
through an enthusiastic teaching practice in order to
cultivate their motivation for the subject.
Finally, a high
variety of emotion display modes within each
cultural-educational context were detected, which could be
traced back to different factors on the individual level, such
as personality (Matsumoto, 2006). This supports the findings
resulting from a meta-analysis done by Van Hemert, Poortinga,
and van de Vijver (2007) who have found several moderators
that reduced the cross-cultural variance in emotion variables.
A particular relevant factor influencing the display mode of
emotions and the quality of the TSR seems to be the dedication
for teaching in general and teaching in teacher education
specifically. As the comparative case-analysis has shown,
teachers who identified strongly with the teaching profession,
reported closer relationships with their students and also
expressed more intensive positive emotions. Furthermore,
position at university and prior teaching experience (in HE
and at school) also appeared to contribute to the quality of
the TSR and the display of emotions.
5.
Conclusion: Study limitations and implications
The results of the
present study have significant implications particularly for
research in the internationalization of HE. While there is
extensive literature on the experiences of international
students in HE, these experiences are under-explored for HE
faculty (Bedenlier & Zawacki-Richter, 2015; Shimmi, 2014).
Our interview accounts have revealed different modes of
forming relationships with students and displaying emotions in
teaching, depending on the cultural-educational context. There
was also evidence that new teachers from a cultural background
different from the local context can feel insecure about how
to behave in an unfamiliar classroom environment. As reported
in Volet and Jones’ (2012) review of literature, scholars
teaching in another country may need support to adapt to new
and culturally different teaching environments. Many
strategies may support their adaptation. For example, in their
first semester, an international and a national scholar could
conduct joint courses, which would allow the scholars to share
how students and teachers typically interact in the respective
countries. In doing so, the international scholar could
develop a sense of the “academic habitus” that prevails in the
host country. Reflection groups that consist of international
teachers and at least one national teacher meeting regularly
to exchange and discuss experiences may be useful. Darwin and
Palmer (2009) introduced the idea of “mentoring circles”,
which they present as a form of group mentoring aimed at staff
development in a HE context. Mentoring circles could be
appropriate environments to assist international scholars to
adapt to their new work environment. More generally, Luna and
Cullen (1995) describe mentoring as a key principle in
empowering faculty, with special strategies for mentoring
minority faculty (e.g., scholars with a different cultural
background).
Helping new
academics from a different cultural-educational background to
adapt to local teaching practices is important but it is
particularly critical in the field of teacher education. This
is because teacher educators do not only teach but are also
expected to model teaching behaviour to prospective teachers.
Therefore they should be aware, not only of appropriate
teaching practices in the HE field, but also of those expected
in the school context. For example, they should become
familiar about the feedback practices in the new country (how
is it expected to communicate satisfaction with a students’
achievement, whether in an emotionally intense way or in a
more reserved way?), and about the degree of informality or
formality in teacher-student interactions at school.
Furthermore, as bridging educational theory with educational
practice (Garcia-Aracil, 2012) is an important contributing
factor to study satisfaction, teacher educators coming from
another cultural-educational background should gain in-depth
understanding of the educational practices of the new country.
More generally, fostering socio-emotional competence (Jennings
& Greenberg, 2009) in teacher educators, for example by
analyzing classroom instructional and interaction practices
through videos (for schoolteachers, see i.e. Seidel et al.,
2011), appears an important avenue in the professionalizing of
teacher educators.
The finding of
differences in the perceived-as-appropriate display rules of
emotions in educational settings, as well as the quality of
the TSR, suggests that teaching strategies cannot be
considered as techniques that can simply be transferred to
different cultural-educational settings. International
educators need to become aware of the frequently unexpressed
and implicit cultural specifics of teaching in HE classrooms.
Awareness of cultural specifics of their own familiar
educational environments as well as those of the new
environment, would not only promote teaching quality from the
outset but also teachers’ and students’ satisfaction in the
teaching learning process. Diener and Diswas-Diener’s (2008)
research has found that satisfaction with relationships is the
most important predictor for life-satisfaction. Developing
competence in a range of cultural appropriate display rules
can, thus, be regarded as an important factor for maintaining
teachers’ wellbeing (Woods, 2010) in international education
settings at home or abroad (for a critical discussion on the
increased emphasis on emotions and emotional well-being in
education, see for example Ecclestone & Hayes, 2009;
Ecclestone, 2012).
Many directions
for future research emerge from the present study.
Methodologically, there is a need for more situated approaches
to explore teacher emotions in the classroom. For example,
systematic (video-) observations in concrete teaching
situations, combined with subsequent stimulated recall
interviews, would generate rich insights on the
cultural-specifics of teacher-student interactions across
countries, as well as culturally diverse classroom settings.
Video-observations could be used together with teacher
questionnaires (e.g. using the “Display Rule Assessment
Inventory” adapted to the HE context; see Mendzheritskaya and
Hansen 2013), following a mixed-methods approach (Creswell
& Plano-Clark, 2011). The present study relied on
self-reports of teachers, which may have drawn some responses
that were affected by social desirability bias to some extent.
Furthermore our study addressed emotions and relationships
directly. Other research – for example the study by Postareff
and Lindblom-Ylänne (2011) – did not explore directly HE
teachers’ emotions, but issues related to emotions
nevertheless emerged from the answers given to other interview
questions. Both ways of exploring emotions are possible
avenues in emotion research and the appropriateness of the
chosen approach depends on the research aim and the sample
(e.g in terms of their willingness and ability to reflect
educational practices including the own emotional and mental
processes). Future studies should combine such subjective
measures with more objective measures, regardless of whether
emotions are assessed through direct or indirect questioning
in the subjective-coined part of the study.
It also seems
necessary to develop quantitative measure that would make it
possible to assess the multidimensionality of the quality of
the TSR in HE (Hagenauer & Volet, 2014c). This could
involve the development of coding schemes to record systematic
observations of teacher-student interactions; coding schemes
that would allow inferences to be drawn on the quality of the
TSR. Furthermore, correlational studies should follow as well
as longitudinal studies exploring how display modes might
affect the establishment of relationships and vice versa.
In terms of
limitations, the present study was conducted with teacher
educators, which suggests caution in generalizing to
university teachers in other domains. As argued by Alheit
(2009), teaching practices vary across domains, departments,
and institutions due to differences in the academic habitus.
The impact of moderating variables should therefore be
examined systematically in future studies, for example, the
teaching domain (e.g. soft versus hard sciences), teachers’
role and status in the university (professor, lecturer,
PhD-student with teaching duties, casual) (Richardson &
Radloff, 2014), the amount and quality of prior teaching
experience (Meanwell & Kleiner, 2014), and the value
placed on teaching at the respective university. The latter
might also influence teachers’ identification as a teacher and
his/her motivation for teaching, which might in turn impact on
the emotional labour required in teaching (Visser-Wijnveen,
Stes, & Van Petegem, 2014). By taking these factors into
account, variability in emotion display and the relationship
quality in different cultural-educational contexts could be
better understood. On an interpersonal level, gender,
personality (e.g., the Big Five or people’s idiocentric vs.
allocentric tendencies) as well as ethnicity and cultural
background should be accounted for. In terms of cultural
background, DeCuir-Gunby and Williams-Johnson (2014) discussed
people’s difficulties to read the expression of emotions of
culturally distant others. This is an important issue if
international students are taught in HE classrooms or if
teachers teach in a foreign or ethnically unfamiliar context.
In addition, by applying an autobiographical approach it would
be possible to explore how the past experiences or the
“personal history”, as coined by Day and Leitch (2001), could
contribute to our understanding of emotion display and
TSR-building processes in HE settings.
Finally, some
cultures may be more heterogeneous than others. In our study,
the Australian sample comprised a few immigrants, while the
German teacher educators were all local. Furthermore, it is
well known that equating country with culture is problematic
(e.g. Matsumoto et al., 1998; Volet & Jones, 2012),
therefore there should be caution in the interpretation of the
findings, keeping in mind that educational practices are
constantly changing in increasingly global environments, and
most importantly also reflect individual preferences.
Concluding, as observed by Van Hemert et al. (2007), studies
on cultural issues in HE teaching should go beyond frequently
made comparisons between individualistic and collectivistic
cultures or countries. This study revealed a high variation of
cultural-educational practices within two so-called
“individualistic” countries (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005).
This calls for a reconceptualization of cross-cultural
research on the relationship between emotion display and the
TSR in HE teaching contexts.
Keypoints
Insight into university teachers’ perceptions of
the characteristics of quality TSR and appropriate emotion
display is essential in the context of the fast developing
internationalization of higher education.
This study revealed major qualitative differences
in the display of positive and negative emotions in two
distinct cultural-educational contexts.
The study also unveiled differences across
cultural-educational contexts in the importance given by
teachers to keeping professional and formal working
relationship with students or alternatively showing
informality and caring in the interpersonal TSR.
International scholars and teachers, like
mobility students, need support to adapt to unfamiliar
cultural-educational environments.
References
Alheit, P. (2009). Die symbolische Macht des
Wissens. Exklusionsmechanismen des
universitären Habitus [The Symbolic Power of Knowledge.
Mechanism of Exclusion of the Academic Habitus]. Presentation held at the
University of Heidelberg (3.6.2009).
Altbach, P. G., & Knight, J. (2007). The
internationalization of higher education: Motivations and
realities. Journal of
Studies in International Education, 11 (3), 290-305. doi: 10.1177/1028315307303542
Barber, L. K., Grawitch, M. J., Carson, R. L.,
& Tsouloupas, C. N. (2010.) Costs and benefits of
supportive versus disciplinary emotion regulation strategies
in teachers. Stress and
Health, 27,
e173-e187. doi: 10.1002/smi.1357.
Bartram, B. (2015). Emotion as a student resource
in higher education. British
Journal of Educational Studies, 63 (1), 67-84. doi: 0.1080/00071005.2014.980222
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The
need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a
fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
Beard, C., Clegg,
S., & Smith, K. (2007). Acknowledging the affective in
higher education. British
Educational Research Journal, 33 (2), 235-252. doi:
10.1080/01411920701208415
Bedenlier, S.,
& Zawacki-Richter, O. (2015). Internationalization of
higher education and the impacts on academic faculty members.
Research in Comparative
and International Education, 10 (2), 185-201.
Online first: 10.1177/1745499915571707
Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2012). The
construction of emotion in interactions, relationships, and
cultures. Emotion
Review, 4
(3), 221-229. doi: 10.1177/1754073912439765
Brennan, R. L., &
Prediger, D. J. (1981). Coefficient Kappa: Some uses, misuses,
and alternatives. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 41, 687-699. doi:
10.1177/001316448104100307
Bortz, J., & Döring, N. (2006). Forschungs- und
Untersuchungsplanung. Heidelberg: Springer.
Chen, Y.-H., & Kristjansson, K. (2011).
Private feelings, public expression: Professional jealousy and
the moral practice of teaching. Journal of Moral
Education, 40
(3), 349-358. doi: 10.1080/03057240.2011.596336
Cranton, P., & Carusetta, E. (2004).
Perspectives on authenticity in teaching. Adult Education
Quarterly, 55 (5), 5-22. doi: 10.1177/0741713604268894
Creswell, J. W., & Plano Clark, V. L.
(2011). Designing and
conducting mixed methods research (2nd ed.).
Los Angeles: Sage.
Darwin, A., & Palmer, E. (2009).
Mentoring circles in higher education. Higher Education Research
& Development, 28 (2), 125-136. doi: 10.1080/07294360902725017
Day, C., & Leitch, R.
(2001). Teachers’ and teacher educators’ lives: The role of
emotion. Teaching and
Teacher Education, 17, 403-415. doi:
10.1016/S0742-051X(01)00003-8
DeCuir-Gunby, J., & Williams-Johnson, M. R.
(2014). The influence of culture on emotions. Implications for
education. In R. Pekrun, & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of
emotions in education (pp. 539-557). New York:
Routledge.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of
self-determination research. Rochester: University of
Rochester Press.
Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the
mysteries of psychological wealth. Malden, MA:
Blackwell Publishing.
Ecclestone, K. (2012). From emotional to
psychological well-being to character education: Challenging
policy discourses of behavioural science and “vulnerability”.
Research Papers in
Education, 27 (4), 463-480.
doi: 10.1080/02671522.2012.690241
Ecclestone, K., & Hayes, D. (2009). Changing
the subject: the educational implications of developing
emotional well-being. Oxford
Review of Education, 35 (3), 371-389. doi: 10.1080/03054980902934662
Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2001). Norms for
experiencing emotions in different cultures: Inter-and
intranational differences.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (5), 869-885.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.869
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003).
Appraisal processes in emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R.
Scherer, & H. Hill Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective
sciences (pp. 572-595). Oxford: University Press.
Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2010).
Social functions of emotions. In M. Lewis, J. M.
Haviland-Jones, & L. Feldman Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions
(3rd ed.) (pp. 456-468). New York: Guilford Press.
Fischer, A. H., Manstead, A. S. R., Evers, C.,
Timmers, M., & Valk, G. (2004). Motives and norms
underlying emotion regulation. In P. Phillippot & R. S.
Feldman (Eds.), The
regulation of emotion (pp.
187-210). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Frenzel, A. (2014). Teacher emotions. In
R. Pekrun, & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of
emotions in education (pp. 494-519). New York:
Routledge.
Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Luedtke, O.,
Pekrun, R., & Sutton, R. (2009). Emotional transmission in
the classroom: Exploring the relationship between teacher and
student enjoyment. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 101, 705-716. doi:
10.1037/a0014695
Garcia-Aracil, A. (2012). A comparative analysis
of study satisfaction among young European higher education
graduates. Irish Educational Studies,
31, 223-243. doi:
10.1080/03323315.2012.660605
Garner, P. W. (2010). Emotional competence
and its influences on teaching and learning. Educational
Psychology Review, 22, 297-321. doi: 0.1007/s10648-010-9129-4
Gates, G. S. (2000). The socialization of
feelings in undergraduate education: A study of emotional
management. College Student Journal, 34 (4), 485-504.
Gläser-Zikuda, M. & Mayring, Ph. (2003). A
qualitative approach to learning emotions at school. In Ph.
Mayring, & Ch. v. Rhöneck (Eds.), Learning Emotions. The
influence of affective factors on classroom learning (pp.
103-126). Berlin: Peter Lang.
Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation.
In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. Feldman Barrett
(Eds.), Handbook of
Emotions (3rd ed.) (pp. 497-512). New York:
Guilford Press.
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation:
Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology,
39 (3),
281-291. doi:
10.1017.S0048577201393198
Hagenauer, G., Hascher, T., & Volet, S. E.
(2015). Teacher
emotions in the classroom: associations with students’
engagement, discipline in the classroom and the interpersonal
teacher-student relationship. European Journal of
Psychology of Education, 30 (4), 385-403. doi:
10.1007/s10212-015-0250-0
Hagenauer, G.,
& Volet, S. E. (2014a). “I don't think I could,
you know, just teach without any emotion”: Exploring the
nature and origin of university teachers’ emotions. Research Papers in
Education, 29
(2), 240-262. doi: 10.1080/02671522.2012.754929
Hagenauer, G., &
Volet, S. E. (2014b). “I don’t
hide my feelings, even though I try to”: Insight into teacher
educator emotion display.
Australian Educational Researcher, 41 (3), 261-281. doi:
10.1007/s13384-013-0129-5
Hagenauer, G., &
Volet, S. E. (2014c). Student-teacher relationship at university: An
important yet under-researched field. Oxford Review of
Education, 40 (3), 370-388. doi:
10.1080/03054985.2014.921613
Hascher, T., & Hagenauer, G. (2016).
Openness to theory and its importance for pre-service
teachers’ self-efficacy, emotions, and classroom behaviour in
the teaching practicum. International
Journal of Educational Research, 77, 15-25. doi:
10.1016/j.ijer.2016.02.003
Hastings, W. (2008). I felt so guilty: emotions
and subjectivity in school-based teacher education. Teachers and Teaching:
Theory and Practice, 14 (5-6), 497-513. doi:
10.1080/13540600802583655
Hatfield, E., Bensman, L., Thornton, P. D., &
Rapson, R. L. (2014). New perspectives on emotional contagion:
a review of classic and recent research on facial mimicry and
contagion. Interpersona,
8 (2), 159-179. doi:10.5964/ijpr.v8i2.162
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart:
Commercialization of human feelings. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G. J. (2005). Cultures and
organization: Software of the mind. London: McGraw-Hill.
Isenbarger, L., &
Zembylas, M. (2006). The emotional labour of caring in
teaching. Teaching and
Teacher Education, 22, 120-134. doi:
10.1016/j.tate.2005.07.002
Jennings, P. A., & Greenberg, M. T.
(2009). The prosocial classroom: Teacher social and emotional
competence in relation to student and classroom outcomes. Review of Educational
Research, 79, 491-525.
10.3102/0034654308325693
König,
J., & Blömeke, S. (2013). Germany. In J. Schwille, L.
Ingvarson, & Holdgreve-Resendez (Eds.), TEDS-M Encyclopedia.
A guide to teacher
education context, structure, and quality assurance in 17
countries. Retrieved from
http://www.iea.nl/fileadmin/user_upload/Publications/Electronic_versions/TEDS-M_Encyclopedia.pdf
Koopman-Holm, B., & Matsumoto, D. (2011).
Values and display rules for specific emotions. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 42, 355-371. doi: 10.1177/0022022110362753
Lazarus, R. S. (1999). Stress and emotion. A new
synthesis. London: Free Association Books.
Luna, G., & Cullen, D. L. (1995). Empowering the faculty.
Mentoring redirected and renewed. ASHE-ERIC Higher
Education Report No. 3. Washington, D. C.: The George
Washington University, Graduate School of Education and Human
Development.
Lunenberg, M., Korthagen,
F., & Swennen, A. (2007). The teacher educator as role
model. Teaching and
Teacher Education, 23, 586-601. doi:
10.1016/j.tate.2006.11.001
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture
and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and
motivation. Psychological
Review, 98, 224-233. doi: 10.1037/0033295X98.2.224
Matsumoto, D. (2006). Are cultural differences in
emotion regulation mediated by personality traits? Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 37, 421-437.
doi: 10.1177/0022022106288478
Matsumoto, D., Takeuchi, S., Andayani, S.,
Kouznetsova, N., & Krupp. D. (1998). The contribution of
individualism vs. collectivism to cross-national differences
in display rules. Asian
Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 147-165. doi: 10.1111/1467-839X.00010
Mayring, P. (2000). Qualitative content analysis.
Forum Qualitative Social
Research, 1 (2). Retrieved from http://www.qualitative-research.net/index.php/fqs/article/view/1089/2385
Meanwell, E., & Kleiner, S. (2014). The
emotional experience of first-time teaching: Reflections from
graduate instructors, 1997-2006. Teaching Sociology, 42 (1),
17-27. doi: 10.1177/0092055X13508377
Mendzheritskaya,
J., & Hansen, M. (2013, August). Shall I show my anger?
Display rules in lecturer-student interaction in Germany and
Russia. Paper presented at the 15th Biennal EARLI
conference. Munich, Germany.
Mendzheritskaya, J., Hansen, M., & Horz, H.
(2015). Emotional display rules at universities in Russia
and Germany. Russian Psychological Journal, 12 (4).
Mesquita, B. (2007). Emotions are culturally
situated. Social
Science Information, 46, 410-415. doi:
10.1177/05390184070460030107
Moore, S., & Kuol, N. (2007). Matters of the
heart: Exploring the emotional dimensions of educational
experience in recollected accounts of excellent teaching. International Journal
of Academic Development, 12 (2), 87-98. doi:
10.1080/13601440701604872
Moran, C. M., Diefendorff, J. M., & Greguras,
G. J. (2013). Understanding emotional display rules at work
and outside of work: The effects of country and gender. Motivation and Emotion,
37, 323-334. doi:
10.1007/s11031-012-9301-x
Neumann, A. (2006). Professing passion: emotion
in scholarship of professors at research universities. American Educational
Research Journal, 43
(3), 381-424. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/4121764
Newberry, M., Gallant, A., & Riley, P.
(Eds.). (2013). Emotion
and school: Understanding how the hidden curriculum
influences relationships, leadership, teaching, and
learning. UK: Emerald.
Nias, J. (1989). Primary teacher talking.
A study of teaching as work. London: Routledge.
Oplatka, I. (2007). Managing emotions in
teaching: Towards an understanding of emotion displays and
caring as nonprescribed role elements. Teachers College Record,
109 (6), 1374-1400.
Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A.
S. R. (2005). Emotion
in social relations. New York: Psychology Press.
Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of
achievement emotions: assumptions, corollaries, and
implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology
Review, 18, 315-341. doi: 10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9
Pillen, M., Beijaard, D., & den Brok, P.
(2013). Professional identity tensions of beginning teachers.
Teachers and Teaching:
Theory and Practice, 19 (6), 660-678. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2013.827455
Postareff, L., &
Lindblom-Ylänne, S. (2011). Emotions and confidence within teaching in higher
education. Studies in
Higher Education, 36, 799-813. doi:
10.1080/03075079.2010.483279
Quinlan, K. M. (2016).
How emotions matters in four key relationships in teaching and
learning in higher education. College Teaching.
Online first: doi: 10.1080/87567555.2015.1088818
Ria, L., Sève, C.,
Saury, J., Theureau, J., & Durand, M. (2003). Beginning
teachers’ situated emotions: A study of first classroom
experiences. Journal of
Education for Teaching, 29 (3), 219-233. doi: 10.1080/0260747032000120114
Richardson, S., & Radloff. A. (2014). Allies
in learning: Critical insights into the importance of
staff-student interactions in university education. Teaching in Higher
Education, 19
(6), 603-615. doi: 10.1080/13562517.2014.901960
Safdar, S., Friedlmeier, W., Matsumoto, D., Yoo,
S. H., Kwantes, C., Kakai, H., & Shigemasu, E. (2009).
Variations in emotional display rules within and across
cultures: A comparison between Canada, USA, and Japan. Canadian Journal of
Behavioural Science, 41 (1), 1-10. doi:
10.1037/a0014387
Schutz, P. A. (2014). Inquiry on teachers’
emotion. Educational
Psychologist, 49, 1-12. doi:
10.1080/00461520.2013.864955
Schutz, P. A., & Zembylas, M. (Eds.). (2011). Advances in teacher
emotion research. The impact on teachers’ lives. Heidelberg:
Springer.
Schwarz, S. H., & Ros, M. (1995). Values in the west: A theoretical and empirical
challenge to the individualism-collectivism cultural
dimension. World
Psychology, 1, 99-122.
Seidel,
T.,
Stürmer, K., Blomberg, G., Kobarg, M., & Schwindt, K.
(2011). Teacher learning from analysis of videotaped classroom
situations: Does it make a difference whether teachers observe
their own teaching or that or others? Teaching and Teacher
Education, 27,
259-267. doi: 10.106/j.tate.2010.08.009
Shimmi, Y. (2014). Experiences of japanese
visiting scholars in the united states: An exploration of
transition. PhD, Boston College, 2014. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/2345/3783
Sutton, R. E., & Wheatley, K. F. (2003).
Teachers’ emotions and teaching: a review of the literature
and directions for future research. Educational Psychology
Review, 15, 327-358. doi: 10.1023/A:1026131715856
Titsworth, S., McKenna, T. P., Mazer, J. P., &
Quinlan, M. M. (2013). The bright side of emotion in the
classroom: Do teachers’ behaviors predict students’ enjoyment,
hope, and pride? Communication
Education, 62 (2), 191-209. doi: 10.1080/03634523.2013.763997
Timostsuk, I., & Ugaste, A. (2012). The role of
emotions in student teachers’ professional identity. European Journal of
Teacher Education, 35 (4), 421-433. doi: 10.1080/02619768.2012.662637
Trigwell, K. (2012).
Relations between teachers’ emotions in teaching and their
approaches to teaching in higher education. Instructional Science,
40, 607-621. doi: 10.1007/s1125-011-9192-3
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H.
(2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 90, 288-307. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288
Van Hemert, D. A., Poortinga, Y. H., & van de
Vijver, F. J. R. (2007). Emotion and culture: A Meta-analysis.
Cognition and Emotion,
21 (5), 913-943. doi: 10.1080/02699930701339293
Visser-Wijnveen, G. J., Stes, A., & Van Petegem,
P. (2014). Clustering teachers’ motivations for teaching. Teaching in Higher
Education, 19 (6), 644-656. doi:
10.1080/13562517.2014.901953
Volet, S. (2001). Understanding learning and
motivation in context: A multi-dimensional and multi-level
cognitive-situative perspective. In S. Järvelä, & S. Volet
(Eds.)., Motivation in
learning contexts: Theoretical advances and methodological
implications (pp. 57-82). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.
Volet, S. E.,
& Jones, C. (2012). Cultural transitions in higher
education: Individual adaptation, transformation and
engagement. In S. Karabenick, & T. Urdan (Eds.), Advances in achievement
and motivation series (Vol. 17): Transitions across schools
and cultures (pp. 241-284). Bingley, UK:
Emerald.
Walker, C.,
& Gleaves, A. (2016). Constructing the caring higher
education teacher: A theoretical framework. Teaching and Teacher
Education, 54, 65-76. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2015.11.013
Woods, C. (2010). Employee Wellbeing in the
higher education workplace: A role for emotion scholarship. Higher Education, 60,
171-185. doi:
10.1007/s10734-009-9293-y
Yuu, K. (2010). Expressing emotions in teaching:
Inducement, suppression, and disclosure as caring profession.
Educational Studies in
Japan. International
Yearbook, 5,
63-75.
Zhang, Q., & Zhang, J. (2013). Instructors’
positive emotions: Effects on student engagement and critical
thinking in U.S. and Chinese classrooms. Communication Education,
62 (4). 395-411. doi: 10.1080/03634523.2013.828842
Zhang, Q., & Zhu, W. (2008). Exploring
emotion in teaching: Emotional labor, burnout, and
satisfaction in Chinese higher education. Communication Education,
57 (1), 105-133.
doi: 10.1080/03634520701586310
[1] Corresponding
author: Gerda Hagenauer;
University of Bern, Institute of Educational Science,
Fabrikstrasse 8, 3012 Bern, Switzerland; email: gerda.hagenauer@edu.unibe.ch Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.14786/flr.v4i3.236