Dynamics of Team Reflexivity after
Feedback
Catherine Gabelicaa,
Piet Van den Bosscheab, Mien Segersa,
Wim Gijselaersa
a
Maastricht University, The Netherlands
bAntwerp
University,
Belgium
Article received 15
January 2014 / revised 11 February 2014 / accepted 5 June
2014 / available online 18 June 2014
Abstract
A great deal of work has been generated on
feedback in teams and has shown that giving performance
feedback to teams is not sufficient to improve performance. To
achieve the potential of feedback, it is stated that teams
need to proactively process this feedback and thus
collectively evaluate their performance and strategies, look
for alternatives, and make clear decisions about ways to
tackle their task. This concept of team reflexivity has been
commonly described as a sequence of behaviours, which relative
importance has not been demonstrated. Further, empirical
research investigating the dynamic aspects of reflexivity has
been scarce. This study sought to explore how reflexivity
evolves over time and at which moments of the team interaction
it is related to team performance. Thirty-two student dyads
participated to a cognitively complex task (flight simulation)
over four performance episodes comprising action phases
followed by transition (feedback) phases. High interdependence
between participants (pilots and co-pilots) was ensured
through the distribution of complementary knowledge in the
dyads. The results showed that teams seldom engaged in full
cycles of reflective behaviours. When looking into individual
behaviours, teams exhibited more reflective behaviours during
action over time, while their reflective behaviours during
feedback did not change, demonstrating a suboptimal feedback
processing as time goes by. Additionally, it was demonstrated
that teams were capable to learn from their past and act upon
feedback to better subsequent team performance but also that
initial performance acts as a trigger to future reflective
behaviours.
Keywords: teams, team learning,
feedback, team reflexivity, team performance
1.
Introduction
Small group work has
gradually progressed to being one of the dominant approaches in
the domain of learning and instruction and professional
development (e.g., Kirschner, 2009). Collaborative learning is
one of the most successful and widespread instructional practice
implemented in schools and universities (e.g., Dillenbourg,
1999, Johnson & Johnson, 1992). Similarly, work teams have
become a central element in the functioning of organisations in
many domains (e.g., health care, military, and aviation) (Salas,
Stagl, Burke, & Goodwin, 2007). Both professional teams and
learning teams face similar challenges inherent to collaboration
and joint understanding (Barron, 2000; Järvelä, Volet, &
Järvenoja, 2010). Specifically, in both environments,
interdependent team members need to interact and communicate
effectively, share knowledge and experiences, and capitalise
each other’s skills and resources to successfully complete a
common task (e.g., Johnson, Johnson, & Stanne, 2000; Salas,
Dickinson, Converse, & Tannenbaum, 1992). Crucially, recent
work has shown that teams that engage in team learning processes
and learn how to work effectively are more likely to succeed
(e.g., Dochy, Gijbels, Raes, & Kyndt, 2014; Van der Haar,
Segers, & Jehn, 2013, Veestraeten, Kyndt, & Dochy,
2014). Team learning has been defined as “an ongoing process of
reflection and action’’ (Edmondson, 1999, p.353) during which
teams reflect on their own prior activities and consequently
plan adjustments for future practice (see Decuyper et al., 2010,
for a review). Although scholars in these areas have tended to
remain isolated within their own disciplines despite obvious
overlaps in research interests, they generally agree that team
learning processes do not occur naturally (Johnson &
Johnson, 1992; Rummel & Spada, 2005; Sims, Salas, &
Burke, 2005). The awareness that not all teams learn, and as a
consequence may reach substandard group performance, raises the
need to outline deliberate interventions to build learning in
teams. More and more, new research interests focus on what can
be done to leverage learning in teams and improve their
performance (e.g., Decuyper, Dochy, & Van den Bossche, 2010;
Salas, Stagl, & Burke, 2004). Despite these renewed efforts,
it seems that potential leverage points (such as training or the
provision of feedback) calibrated for teams need to be better
specified and validated (Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006).
Giving teams feedback on
their team process and performance has been identified as a
leverage point that shapes team learning and can improve team
performance (Gabelica, Van den Bossche, Segers, &
Gijselaers, 2012; Johnson & Johnson, 2002; London &
Sessa, 2006; Phielix, Prins, & Kirschner, 2010). In school
and beyond, teams need feedback to monitor and regulate their
work (Hattie & Timperley, 2007). Previous theoretical work
on feedback provided by external agents at the team level of
analysis (e.g., Goodman, Wood, & Hendrickx, 2004; London
& Sessa, 2006) suggests that to achieve changes in team
learning and performance, teams need to process received
feedback, be receptive to this feedback, understand its value,
and actively engage in collaborative activities during which
they use feedback cues to make improvements. Nevertheless,
empirical work on the value of active feedback processing to the
mere reception of feedback in teams has considerably lagged
behind theoretical development (Hattie & Timperley, 2007).
This feedback processing has yet to be empirically examined
(Gabelica et al., 2012). More specifically, the more interesting
question about feedback effectiveness is rather how learning
naturally happens during the team feedback process and how
effective are these learning processes (e.g., Adcroft, 2011).
Moreover, previous work in both team and collaborative learning
research leaves much about the dynamics of feedback processing
in teams unspecified, such as 1) how do teams respond to
repeated (external) feedback in dialogue over the course of
ongoing activities, and 2) when (i.e., at which point in time)
are these behaviours related to effective learning and
performance. There is a general agreement across disciplines
that we should consider feedback loops in which behavioural
changes resulting from each cycle are inputs in cycles that
follow (e.g., Soller, Monés, Jermann, & Mühlenbrock, 2005)
but this is rarely reflected in research designs (e.g., Ilgen,
Hollenbeck, Johnson, & Jundt, 2005).
Concerning how teams process
feedback, we propose that they do so by performing shared
reflective activities, that is by collectively discussing and
reflecting upon their functioning (e.g., Schippers, Den Hartog,
& Koopman, 2007). These activities are core building blocks
of team learning. Specifically, it has been shown that
reflective teams evaluate their performance and strategies, look
for alternatives to consider situations, and make decisions
about new ways to tackle their task. The concept of team
reflexivity, as proposed in organisational psychology, mirrors
these activities (West, Garrod, & Carletta, 1997). In
educational settings, generic forms of intra-group reflection
such as collective/social metacognition (McCarthy & Garavan,
2008), reflection (Edmondson, 1999), collaborative reflection
(Morris & Stew, 2007; Yukawa, 2006), peer reflection, or
reflective self-explanation (Rummel, Spada & Hauser, 2009)
have been increasingly used. This recent research area is an
extension of the work on individual reflection or reflective
practice (e.g., Boud, Keogh, & Walker, 1985) that adds
interactions and communication with peers to the learning
process. Many authors agree that team reflexivity (in any
generic sense) allows teams to reach a more accurate
understanding of their task and, as a result, better performance
(e.g., McCarthy & Garavan, 2008; Schippers, Homan, & Van
Knippenberg, 2013). Although the very recent research strand on
team reflexivity acknowledges the importance of the dynamics of
team performance when considering team reflexivity, the
empirical work is only beginning to consider under which
circumstances team reflexivity relates to changing performance,
but not in contexts with systematic performance data on which to
reflect (Schipper et al., 2013). Across disciplines, external
and specific feedback is not systematically part of the
reflective process while it is usually agreed that reflection
can only occur if people have accurate knowledge about their
current and desired learning state (Hattie, 2013). Also, the
relation between time and timing of reflexivity and team
performance remains in question, such as does reflecting right
from the start of a team activity help the team get started and
allows later success or does sustained reflection after events
later in a team’s life also matter for sustained performance?
Thus, when teams process
feedback appears as a gap in both feedback and reflexivity
research. Previous research on feedback and team performance
suggests that feedback effects are not static but dynamic (e.g.,
McGrath, 1993); it cannot be understood as a single-cycle linear
path from inputs (e.g., feedback) through outcomes (e.g., team
performance). In the same vain, how teams learn from feedback
should also be considered with a dynamic glance (Ilgen et al.,
2005)
The purpose of the present
study is therefore to address the above-mentioned gaps by
shedding light on how reflective behaviours relate to
performance over a period of time in a complex, fast-paced, and
high-workload situation in which two individuals with
distributed information have to keep on learning to achieve
success. Specifically, the following questions are explored: 1)
how do teams naturally overtly reflect when provided with
feedback depicting their performance, 2) how do reflective
behaviours grow over time during and/or after action, 3) how
does the timing at which team reflexivity occurs relate to
performance?
1.1 Feedback interventions
Prior to addressing
feedback in team settings, it is critical to briefly solicit
input from multiple disciplines to better understand how the
much more substantial body of research on feedback given to
individuals have shaped the feedback concept in teams.
In the learning sciences,
feedback is an instructional practice that is expected to
enhance motivation and learning (Mulder & Ellinger, 2013;
Shute, 2008). Learning scientists have acknowledged feedback as
a key characteristic of quality teaching decades ago in non-team
settings (e.g., Mory, 2003; Shute, 2008, Yang & Carless,
2013). Much of the extensive work on feedback given to
individuals has come to two main conclusions: 1) learners should
be given feedback containing learning information (e.g.,
Duijnhouwer, Prins, & Stokking, 2012; Gibbs & Simpson,
2004) and 2) researchers should consider feedback from the
perspective of the feedback receiver and thus incorporate the
uptake and the receptivity of feedback in the feedback process
(e.g., Boud & Molloy, 2013; Eva et al., 2012). This also
introduces the idea of a feedback process that goes beyond the
provision of feedback (Mulder, 2013). Since feedback is
traditionally part of instructional programs, the drawback of
multi-component interventions is that it is not always possible
to assign behavioural changes to feedback interventions (e.g.,
van der Pol, van den Berg, Admiraal, & Simons, 2008).
Further, most studies concern primary school and high school
students (e.g., Johnson & Johnson, 1993), which raises the
question of the generalizability of findings to higher education
or workplace (adult) learning.
By contrast,
organisational, social, and behavioural psychology have
incorporated feedback delivery in many (semi)experimental
research and extensively investigated its added value with or
without other components (such as goal setting) to human
performance (e.g., Kluger & DeNisi, 1996) while the feedback
process has largely remained a black box. Furthermore, feedback
is often mere “knowledge of performance or results” (e.g.,
performance data of a company or score on a simulation game)
instead of elaborated informational feedback (e.g., Austin,
Kessler, Riccobono, & Bailey, 1996). The learning value of
feedback seems to be a consistent omission. Moreover, this
research tradition has primarily focused on post-secondary
levels.
Taken together these
disciplines have given rise to a new question transcending the
simple question of whether or not feedback is truly effective:
how and under which conditions feedback improves learning and
performance. This concern has been echoed in the relatively
smaller research strand on feedback to teams.
1.2 Feedback to teams
Feedback at the team level
of analysis is defined as the communication of information
provided by (an) external agent(s) concerning actions, events,
processes, or behaviours relative to task completion or teamwork
(Gabelica et al., 2012, London, 2003).
It is widely accepted that
feedback can provide teams with accurate information on their
performance and may steer, motivate, support, and reinforce
future team behaviour. Feedback is considered as a leverage
point in the team's development of a collective view of
expectations and awareness about its behaviours, capabilities,
and skills (London & Sessa, 2006; Prins, Sluijsmans, &
Kirschner, 2006). Research in collaborative learning
environments has highlighted that feedback has the power to draw
the team’s attention to specific aspects of its task and hence
encourage task-related discussion (Johnson & Johnson, 2002).
In the workplace, feedback can also serve as a motivational
trigger. For example, Scott-Young and Samson (2009) showed that
providing teams of managers with performance feedback reinforced
teams’ confidence in themselves and in turn, their performance.
Despite many potential
benefits of feedback delivery, a recent review by Gabelica et
al. (2012) integrated findings from fifty-nine empirical studies
investigating the effects of feedback in teams in educational
and professional settings and showed mixed results.
Approximately one third of the studies did not find support for
its expected positive effect on performance. For example, in a
field experiment, Jung and Sosik (2003) found that giving
feedback to teams performing decision-making tasks had positive
benefits on group members’ collective confidence (i.e.,
collective efficacy and group potency) but not on team
performance. Based on these inconsistent results, analogue to
feedback research in non-team settings, Gabelica and colleagues
(2012) concluded that the key question of whether team feedback
is effective depends on the conditions under which feedback is
given, and not only on feedback as such (e.g., its quality).
Based on educational
research, it can be argued that in addition to factors related
to the feedback giver and environment, feedback receivers have a
critical role to play. Research on team feedback suggests that
teams given feedback will only change if they perceive a
learning need and opportunity and if they attend, interpret, and
act upon feedback (e.g. London & Sessa, 2006; Phielix et
al., 2010). In other words, teams need to proactively process
the content of feedback, and thus invest time and effort into
actively building content-oriented reactions if we expect
visible changes in the way they perform. Yet, how teams process
information cues contained in feedback, and thus what specific
processing behaviours and activities are dynamically related to
performance remains largely unknown (Gabelica et al., 2012).
Although there are few studies on peer feedback exploring the
role of feedback receivers during the feedback process in teams
(e.g. Prins et al., 2006), the interconnections between uptakes
of feedback receivers and ongoing performance are still unclear.
Also, since the success of feedback in terms of an effective
uptake from the receivers depends at least partially on the
feedback quality provided by others, studying the uptake of
standardised feedback (i.e., of constant quality and constant
source) would allow us to isolate the learning effects of
providing feedback.
In sum, there seems to be
an agreement that reaching an intersubjective understanding of
the content of the feedback in teams by discussing what can be
learned and worked out from past experiences is a potent factor
augmenting feedback effectiveness (e.g., Boud et al., 1985).
Despite a lack of direct evidence establishing the benefits of
feedback processing behaviours, the consensus appears to be that
the construct holds enough potential to warrant further
investigation. The recent research strand on team reflexivity
depicting the extent to which teams reflect upon and modify
their functioning informs our understanding of these processing
behaviours (Schippers et al., 2013).
1.3 Team reflexivity
In pedagogy, individual
reflection- or reflective practice- can be traced back to the
early 1900s (Dewey, 1910, 1997) but has been introduced more
extensively into the field of professional learning by Schön
(1983) as professionals’ critical consideration of what they are
working on while they are working on it. On a simple level, one
can consider reflection in the past, present, and future tense.
Schön refers to ‘reflection-in-action’ as analysis in the
present tense (i.e., reflection on the spot) and
‘reflection-on-action’ as analysis in the past tense (i.e.,
review of past actions). Killion and Todnem (1991) underlie a
lack of forward thinking implicit in Dewey’s work and propose
that reflection should also inform future action. Thus, they
added ‘reflection-for-action’ as reflection oriented towards the
future (i.e., identification of guidelines to follow to succeed
in the future).
Reflection as an individual
critical thinking process has been recently extended to a view
of reflection as a collaborative critical thinking process
consisting of cognitive and social interactions between two or
more individuals who examine their experiences to construct
novel intersubjective understandings (Boud et al., 1985; Yukawa,
2006). As such, it is considered as a core team learning
process. Work on team learning has demonstrated that collective
learning can be realised through iterative sequences of action,
reflection, and implementation (Dochy et al., 2014; Edmondson,
1999). In the learning sciences, there are multiple labels
denoting this concept of reflection at the team level. For
example, the following terms have been used: collaborative
reflection (Morris & Stew, 2007; Yukawa, 2006), peer
reflection, reflective self-explanation (Rummel, Spada, &
Hauser, 2009), or collective or social metacognition (McCarthy
& Garavan, 2008). In small group research, principally one
label “team reflexivity” has been introduced by West (1996) as a
set of collaborative reflective behaviours and activities during
which the team objectives, strategies, and processes are
discussed openly. We use the term “team reflexivity” throughout
this paper as a unique label for reflection at the team level.
Originally, the concept of
team reflexivity has not been explicitly connected to the
feedback process. However, it is generally acknowledged that
reflection is enabled by feedback to ensure accuracy in learning
(Hattie, 2013). As a result, team reflexivity can be
conceptualised as ways teams collectively try to extract meaning
and cues for future behaviours from received feedback, generate
intentions and plans, and ultimately decide to act upon
feedback. Thus, when performance feedback that is merely
evaluative is given to teams, the process that follows this
feedback moment might be shared reflection on the task and the
team process. The underlying assumption is that team feedback
gives goal-oriented information but teams are still responsible
for its mindful uptake. It can be argued that reflective teams
consider reasons, rationales, and evidence for this evaluation
of past performance, weigh alternative perspectives to construct
a better understanding of their collective experience that, in
turn, better guides their future action (Yukawa, 2006).
Three behaviours that
reflect complementary dimensions of team reflexivity can hence
be derived from previous work on team reflexivity across
disciplines (e.g., Schippers et al., 2007; Yukawa, 2006): (a)
evaluating present and past performance and strategies, (b)
looking for alternatives, and (c) making decisions. Evaluating
refers to team members reviewing their goals, performance,
strategies, and possible reasons behind success or failures.
Looking for alternatives occurs when teams make an inventory of
possible ways to achieve the task. Finally, making decisions
consists of clearly stating a decision about how to handle the
task differently and acting upon it. Evaluating and looking for
alternatives reflect the capability of the team to be self-aware
of its behaviours and the necessity to make changes. According
to Schippers and colleagues (2007), this is necessary but not
sufficient to engage in change. Teams also need to implement the
adapted actions. This is reflected by our conceptualisation of
‘’making decisions’’ that depicts both the ‘’intention to act’’
and ‘’carrying out the decision’’. Hence, this suggests a
time-ordered sequence of reflective behaviours that might
constitute reflective cycles, although no empirical work
supports the necessity of full three-phase sequences.
Overall, reflective teams
have the ability to uncover why they succeeded or failed, solve
misunderstandings, and correct their future approaches as new
challenges emerge (Tschan, Semmer, Nägele, & Gurtner, 2000;
Wills & Clerkin, 2009). As a consequence, team reflexivity
has been recognised as an important contributor to effective
collaboration and performance (e.g., Kramarski, 2004; Rummel,
Mullins, & Spada, 2012; Schippers, Den Hartog, Koopman,
& van Knippenberg, 2008; Tjosvold, Tang, &West, 2004;
van Ginkel, Tindale, & Knippenberg, 2009). However, in their
review of small group research Moreland and McMinn (2010) draw
attention to 1) the lack of significant relation between
reflexivity and team performance found in some studies (e.g.,
Edmondson, Bohmer, & Pisano, 2001; Savelsbergh, van der
Heijden, & Poell, 2009) and 2) relatively limited evidence
of the effect of reflexivity on team performance (e.g., Lewis,
Belliveau, Herndon, & Keller, 2007; Müller, Herbig, &
Petrovic, 2009). They concluded that reflexivity could be
beneficial to team performance under certain circumstances. In
the learning sciences, a similar trend has been observed:
although team reflexivity in collaborative teams is highly
important for the learning process, it does not always yield
better learning gains (e.g., Prinsen, Terwelb, Zijlstrac, &
Volman, 2013).
Given these mixed results,
limitations of the small but growing research strand on team
reflexivity need to be synthesised. First, reflexivity does not
happen in a vacuum. Teams will eventually adapt their operating
methods and ways of working based on feedback cues from their
environment. We could expect reflexivity to only improve team
performance when teams have access to feedback describing their
objective and accurate performance (Schippers et al, 2013). Yet,
reflexivity is seldom conceptualised as a process augmenting the
effect of feedback on performance (Seibert, 1999). Moreover,
little is known about how people reflect on feedback at the team
level, while there has been empirical evidence of the effect of
reflection upon feedback at the individual level (e.g.,
Duijnhouwer et al., 2012). For example, Anseel, Lievens, and
Schollaert (2009) have tested the effect of feedback augmented
with reflection at the individual level and demonstrated that
the combined use of individual-level feedback and reflection
improved performance better than individual feedback alone. At
the team-level, only one series of studies isolated the effect
of feedback from its combination with reflection in
computer-supported collaborative learning in high-school teams
(Phielix, Prins, & Kirschner, 2010; Phielix, Prins,
Kirschner, Erkens, & Jaspers, 2011). The authors expected
shared self and peer assessment and shared reflection to have
complementary effects. They did not find any significant effect
of reflection alone or of the combined use of feedback and
reflection on objective performance (i.e., grade), but
demonstrated that the joint use of feedback and reflection lead
to higher group process satisfaction and social and cognitive
behaviour. Interestingly, they draw attention to the fact that
feedback (based on peer and self-perceptions) and reflection did
not decrease unrealistic positive perceptions teams generally
have about their own and other performance. This could be a
reason for a lack of effect on objective performance. We do not
know what are the effects of external feedback based on
objective criteria for task achievement.
Second, as in most research
in organisational psychology, the vast majority of small group
research measuring team reflexivity in relation to team
performance has used self-report instruments. Self-report
measures are limited by team members’ level of awareness of
their own behaviours and states and distorting biases such as
social desirability. Calls for studying reflective behaviours in
teams have generally gone unheeded (West, 1996). In the learning
sciences, Dillenbourg, Baker, Blaye, and O’Malley (1996) have
advised researchers to zoom in the ‘black box’ of collaborative
processes. Subsequent to this call, there has been a recent
proliferation of process-oriented research on collaboration
(e.g., De Wever, Schellens, Valcke, & Van Keer, 2005) that
advanced our understanding on interaction features contributing
to more effective learning. Most of this collaborative learning
research strand has focused on individual learning without
explicitly investigating how collaborative processes influence
team performance (e.g., Janssen, Kirschner, Erkens, Kirschner,
& Paas, 2010). Nevertheless, these insights underscore the
value of observational methods to provide crucial information
about the context in which reflective behaviours occur and
relate to team performance (e.g., Chi, 1997; Leicht, Hunter,
Saluja, & Messner, 2010).
Finally, one area in which
our understanding is incomplete across disciplines concerns the
role of time and timing of reflective behaviours and their
relation with team performance (e.g., Ballard, Tschan, &
Waller, 2008; Janssen et al., 2010; Okhuysen & Waller, 2002;
Reimann, 2007; Waller, 1999). There is a general agreement in
the team literature that team performance is the product of
ongoing and recurrent processes and actions (McGrath, 1993).
Marks and colleagues (2001) conceptualise these cycles as
performance episodes. Performance episodes consist of repeated
cycles of action (i.e., when teams perform an activity) and
transition (or ‘interrupts’) phases between actions. These
interrupts are opportunities for teams to stop and reflect about
their progresses for engaging in change (Okhuysen & Waller,
2002). The most common conceptualisation of team reflexivity
does stipulate that shared reflection can occur before, during,
and after a task (West, 2000; Schippers et al., 2007). However,
scholars have generally measured it as an overall working style
(Gurtner, Tschan, Semmer, & Nagele, 2007) or as an
aggregated measure of collaborative activities and have not
differentiated the moments at which it occurs (e.g., Lajoie
& Lu, 2012). Specifically, they have not tested whether team
reflexivity was more or less beneficial in certain phases, in
relation to team performance dynamics, as suggested by certain
authors (Hoeg & Parboteeah, 2006; Janssen et al., 2010;
Schippers et al., 2003). This time-related issue of team
reflexivity is elaborated upon in the following section.
1.3.1
Time and timing of team reflexivity
We identify three primary
issues in understanding the dynamic aspects of team reflexivity.
First, although the importance of dynamic conditions experienced
by teams over time is widely accepted (e.g., Waller, 1999),
empirical work on how team reflexivity changes over time is
missing (e.g., Janssen et al., 2010). On the one hand, it may be
that overt communication is no longer needed as teams improve
their implicit coordination over time, thus decreasing
reflective interactions (e.g., Entin & Serfaty, 1999).
Additionally, teams tend to define their goals and strategies at
an early stage of their work and not to deliberatively review
them after some work has been accomplished (Argote, 1989;
Hackman & Wageman, 2005). Also, in line with arguments from
Schippers and colleagues (2003), reflexivity might decline over
time in diverse teams as viewpoints and perspectives become
incompatible. On the other hand, they suggest that this
declining effect might be reduced by the provision of feedback.
It may be that the availability of accurate performance data
highlighting deficiencies and a sustained task complexity
trigger learning needs for teams perhaps calling for more
reflection over time (Rulke & Rau, 2000). As such, feedback
provision occurring during transition can act as a formal
mechanism, a temporal punctuation likely to encourage reflection
without necessarily giving a predetermined framework to follow
(Okhuysen & Waller, 2002).
Second, the role of timing
of reflective behaviours is similarly not well understood. The
scarce previous research on the question ‘’does reflection
during action and/or transition lead to better performance’’ has
shown mixed results in contexts without explicit feedback. For
example, in a study conducted by Moreland and McMinn (2010),
none of the (scarce) reflective behaviours occurring during
transition was significantly related to changes in team
performance. By contrast, research looking into the impact of
"interrupts" on group processes concluded that these were
triggers of change in groups (Okhuysen & Eisenhardt, 2002).
Team members appear to naturally interrupt their work around the
midpoint of the allocated time for task completion and be more
likely to put into practice strategies they set during these
time outs (Gersick, 1989). Concerning reflection during action,
Moreland and his colleague suggest that it might have more
impact on performance. This reflection-in-action would be more
directly related to the activities team members perform and thus
prevent errors from being committed in real time. Conversely, it
is likely that reflection while performing has a cost especially
in a task that combines active processing of information and
coordinated actions (Kirschner, Paas, & Kirschner, 2009;
Schippers et al., 2013). That is, reflecting while executing a
complex task places an extra burden on teams which may overload
their working memory occasioning less optimal performance
(Kirschner et al., 2009).
Furthermore, it is
generally recognised that what happens in the early part of the
team interaction might provide insight into subsequent
effectiveness (e.g., Eriksen & Dyer, 2004; Kaplan, Laport,
& Waller, 2013). Team decision-making literature has
provided preliminary insights into this issue of timing of
behaviours. It was shown that in teams with distributed
information (i.e., comprising team members holding unique
information), early agreements might harm decision quality
because teams are less focused on exchanging and integrating
distributed information (van Ginkel et al., 2009; van Ginkel
& van Knippenberg, 2009). Accordingly, jumping too early
into task completion might lead to process losses and
performance decline (Mathieu & Rapp, 2008). On the contrary,
it was demonstrated that effective teams share and elaborate
upon distributed information at the beginning of interaction
(van Ginkel at al., 2009). Rulke and Rau (2000) came to a
similar conclusion when examining how teams develop a shared
understanding of ‘who knows what’ in the team (i.e., transactive
memory systems), proved to be an important factor to achieve
better success. They observed that teams with high transactive
memory systems were those whose members shared understandings
and evaluated each other’s expertise early in their team
interactions. In another example from computer-supported
collaborative learning research, Kapur, Voiklis, and Kinzer
(2008) demonstrated that a high quality contribution at the
beginning of a problem solving process had more impact than
those occurring later during team interactions. Therefore, the
temporal pattern within reflective interactions should be taken
into account in further understanding team reflexivity upon
feedback. Also, presently, in the team reflexivity literature
the question ‘’do the three behaviours making up reflexivity
have differential effects on subsequent performance at an early
stage of team interaction’’ remains unanswered. No empirical
work has shown the necessity of a certain order of these
reflective behaviours nor whether, and if so when, certain
reflective behaviours were more conducive to better performance.
For example, if we look into team reflective behaviours
individually, we do not know if evaluating and looking for
alternatives during teams’ first moment of interaction promote
elaboration and understanding of the task whereas making
decisions at an early stage is detrimental to subsequent
performance.
Third, the direction of the
relation between reflexivity and performance can be questioned
(e.g., Janssen et al., 2010). In line with the core assumption
of previous research on team reflexivity, does reflexivity lead
to subsequent better performance? Alternatively or at the same
time, do teams learn from previous performance, and thus reflect
more as a consequence of how they performed previously? Research
still has to prove the theoretical claim that teams can learn
from the past through reflection with clear sight of performance
criteria and information about their attainment. Only recently,
Schippers and colleagues (2013) have given indirect evidence in
this regard. In this study, self-report reflexivity was measured
at two points in time in teams of students working on their
bachelor thesis. This study showed that low-performing teams had
the capability to translate information from performance
feedback into effective task approaches. However, students were
only given a grade and not feedback describing attainment of
specific performance criteria. As suggested by Waller, Gupta,
and Giambatista (2004) the timing of errors and subsequent
behaviours has to be recorded to answer the question of
causality. Further, investigating the timing of reflective
activities in teams can help detect the points at which team
reflexivity occurs and may need to be supported (Lajoie &
Lu, 2012).
1.4 The present study
In the present study, we
seek to understand the dynamics of team reflexivity and the
relation (uni or bi-directional) between team reflexivity and
performance. Therefore, we explore the two following questions.
1) Does the occurrence of team reflexivity augment or decline
over time during action and transition phases of teamwork? 2)
How is the timing at which reflective behaviour occurs related
to performance? Specifically, is reflexivity during action
and/or during transition related to higher performance (a)? Does
each behaviour making up team reflexivity have the same impact
on performance when occurring during teams’ first moment of
interaction (b)?
2.
Method
2.1 Participants
Sixty-four students (32
male and 32 females) were recruited from a university in the
Netherlands and randomly assigned to thirty-two dyads (N = 32).
Their ages ranged from 18 to 29 years, M = 22.3, SD = 2.4.
Participants were not eligible if any of the following exclusion
criteria were present: experience in flight or related
simulations and familiarity with each other. They were either
paired with a same-gender partner (female and male teams, n = 11
and n = 11 respectively) or different-gender partner (mixed
teams, n = 10). By random assignment, half of the sample was
assigned to a role of pilot and the other half to a role of
co-pilot. Subjects participated voluntarily in exchange for
vouchers.
2.2 Task
Participants in the role of
pilot and co-pilot were required to complete four landing
missions of the computer simulation “Microsoft Flight Simulator
X”. The task, a complex, fast-paced, and high-workload
situation, was chosen to stimulate ongoing learning in a
controlled environment. Cognitively complex and interactive
simulation tasks, such as flight-simulations, are commonly used
in team research to investigate processes related to team
performance (e.g., Bowers, Salas, Prince, & Brannick, 1992;
Villado & Arthur, 2013). We did not use this computer
simulation to mimic real-work team environments but rather to
examine a set of theoretical relations (i.e., nomological
network) among constructs within specific and controlled
boundaries: a complex, fast-paced, and high-workload situation
in which team members with unequally distributed information
have to learn from each other to achieve their team goal and
extend their learning to more complex variations of the task
(Marks, 2000). To avoid that good-performing teams would have
less need to learn as a consequence of their reflection on
performance (Schippers et al. 2013), the level of complexity of
the missions increased gradually over time. The abundance of
information teams received before and during the missions and
the high level of interdependence between pilot and co-pilot
ensured a high level of complexity across performance episodes.
In each mission, teams had to follow a predetermined traffic
pattern during which they were required to maintain appropriate
levels of speed, altitude, and a correct configuration of the
airplane. The missions were completed when the team managed to
land safely on the runway. The computer was connected to a
whiteboard on which the game was screened.
2.3 Procedure
The whole session lasted
approximately two and a half hours. After introduction to the
procedure and random assignment to the role of pilot or
co-pilot, participants were individually trained during
forty-five minutes. Items of information necessary for achieving
a good landing were distributed between the team members. Pilots
and co-pilots were seated in separate rooms to study the task
material containing critical role-specific knowledge of piloting
or monitoring the aircraft. The task of the pilot was to fly the
plane and operate the joystick. For that purpose, pilots
received an additional 10-minute hands-on training to practice.
The task of the co-pilot was to control the gas of the plane and
provide the pilot with indications and directions. Only the
co-pilot had the access to the air traffic control (ATC)’s
instructions, given through headphones, and knew how to
interpret the cockpit instruments. After the training,
participants were seated together to complete four landing
missions. Teams had up to fifteen minutes to complete each
mission and were also allowed to restart a mission if they had
crashed. Before each mission the team received a written
description of the flight objectives and the general mission
scenario. Moreover, before starting missions 2, 3, and 4, teams
were given specific performance feedback about their previous
performance. Performance feedback described the attainment of
success criteria such as speed, altitude, rate of descent,
pitch, touchdown, and traffic pattern. The participants were
allowed to communicate freely with one another. All teams were
videotaped.
2.4 Measures
2.4.1
Team
performance
Two performance scores were
computed: the total number of errors during a mission and the
number of times teams crashed. The number of errors was derived
from an instrument rating objective performance criteria (e.g.,
speed, altitude, activation of flaps and landing gear, landing
position) of a good landing approach. This instrument was based
on two sources: firstly, to identify key factors of a good
flight, we performed a task analysis with a flight expert.
Secondly, we used tests that the game itself provides its
players to refine these criteria. Examples of deficiencies
(i.e., errors) included failure to extend the flaps before
landing, to maintain a certain speed interval during descent, to
reduce the speed before touchdown, to keep a constant rate of
descent, to align with the runaway, or to have one touchdown on
the runaway. The total number of potential errors varied in the
four missions. This variation reflects the increasing level of
difficulty of the missions. We chose the number of crashes to
depict one of the most salient manifests of performance for
participants. There were four measurement times in total (i.e.,
T1, T2, T3, and T4).
2.4.2
Categories
of
team reflective behaviours
Team communication was
coded to identify representative behaviours that could be taken
as evidence of team reflexivity (Rourke & Anderson, 2004).
We developed the coding scheme of the present study through a
series of steps assuring its validity and reliability (Schippers
et al., 2007). First, we determined the granularity of the unit
of analysis. The unit of meaning was applied (Rourke, Anderson,
Garrison, & Archer, 2001). Specifically, to consider a
verbal statement a significant unit, we decided that utterances
had to be individual messages (questions or statements) that 1)
were expressed by one team member, 2) dealt with one topic,
idea, or argument chain, 3) reflected one unique behaviour, and
4) related to the topic at hand or the team. Thus, one semantic
feature (unit of meaning) and one activity feature (team member
speaking) were used for segmentation of the communication
content into units (Chi, 1997). As such, as soon as the topic or
the speaker changed, a new behaviour was coded
(Visschers-Pleijers, Dolmans, de Leng, Wolfhagen, & van der
Vleuten, 2006). In addition to verbal statements, one
unambiguous non-verbal behaviour was set as an evidence of one
of the reflective behaviours.
Second, we discriminated
verbal interactions types that typified reflexivity. To do this,
we adapted and expanded the initial framework of team
reflexivity (West, 1996, 2000) and an existing questionnaire
from Schippers and colleagues (2003). Reflexivity was originally
defined as an iterative process including three broader
behaviours, namely reflection, planning, and acting/adapting. As
shown in Table 1, the coding scheme covers three reflective
behaviours: evaluating or reviewing present or past team
performance and strategies, looking for alternatives, and making
decisions. Information directly forwarded from the ATC
(repetitions) and the literal reading of the feedback form were
excluded from the coding.
Table 1
The Coding Scheme for the
Content Analysis of Team Reflexivity
Categories
|
Description |
Examples |
Evaluating or reviewing performance or
strategies |
Statements or questions about team
performance (e.g., whether the team does/did well,
is/was on the right track according to plans or received
instructions), the goal of the mission and its
requirements, actions and strategies (mis)used, reasons
behind success, failure, or problems (e.g., he/she gives
examples of behaviours, task or team strategies that may
explain why they achieved success or encountered
problems during this mission). |
“We are
going in the wrong direction.’’ “We crashed
because we were always too fast.” “Something
went wrong, maybe the nose of the plane went too low?” |
Looking for alternatives |
Suggestions or discussions of alternatives
in how they approached the task (at the task or team
levels) and of the sequence of actions undertaken. In
other words, teams discuss how they could do or could
have done differently. |
“We could have reduced the speed by
pitching up or reducing the throttle.” “We could lower the speed by extending the
flaps, pitching up, or lower the gas.” “We could either make a U-turn either
still try to lower speed and make a sharp descent.” |
Making decisions |
Statements clearly depicting a decision
about a new direction to take or observable behaviours
following a decision. Team members’ utterances depicting
very explicit decisions about the way they were going to
approach the task or work as a team, explicit statements
about the intention to follow decisions made within the
team, and explicit reaction to a decision by an action
(e.g., by pressing the flaps, pulling the gas
controller). |
‘’We are going to make a U-Turn” or “This
time, you look at the speed indicator and I will pitch
down”. |
Third, we ran a pilot study
to test and validate the coding scheme. This lead to
adaptations, clarifications of the reasoning behind the
framework definitions and the boundaries of the units, and the
addition of typical examples. Fourth, we extensively trained two
coders, each blind to the hypotheses of the study, to optimise
reliability and consequently reduce errors in observation. They
were provided with clear examples (of inclusion and exclusion)
of the manifestation of the behaviours and had rating exercises
with multiple rounds and discussions to attain consistency among
coders. Fifth, videotapes were coded with the newly developed
coding scheme. Finally, the two coders coded independently
one-third of the videotapes to estimate interrater reliability
(Cohen’s kappa). Kappas were calculated for all the categories.
These kappas ranged from 0.65 to 0.88, with an average of 0.78,
indicating a ‘substantial’ to ‘almost perfect agreement’ across
the two coders as to the occurrence of the specific behaviours
(Landis & Koch, 1977). Coders and two trainers resolved any
discrepancies.
The research design is
displayed in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Overview of
the phases of the study and behaviours measured at each wave of
data. (see pdf file)
3.
Analyses
For the coding, we used The
Observer® XT 10.5, a computer software aimed for quantitative
analysis of observational data. Videotapes were directly coded
without transcripts. The extent to which teams engaged in each
behaviour was expressed in terms of frequencies of occurrence of
the behaviour of its members for each mission (i.e., action
phase) and for each feedback (transition) phase. Additionally,
we computed an overall team reflexivity score (i.e., aggregation
of the three behaviours) for each time measurement for action
and feedback.
We coded acts on the basis
of utterances of reflective behaviours occurring at the team
level of analysis (i.e., aggregation of individual utterances
for each team). Besides utterances, we examined phases (or
‘’when’’, specifically action or transition, earlier interaction
or later interaction) in which some performance events (i.e.,
crashes and errors) were related to team reflective behaviours.
Behaviours were coded at four points in time during action and
feedback phases (Time 1, Time 2, Time 3, and Time 4) for each
team (N = 32).
4.
Results
In the following section,
we first present an overview of the frequencies of behaviours
considered individually and of the frequencies of sequences
comprising two or three behaviours. Second, we test whether
reflective behaviours change over time during action and
feedback using repeated-measures analyses of variance. Third,
correlations between team reflective behaviours and performance
are examined, more specifically (a) the relations between prior
performance and subsequent team reflexivity and (b) prior team
reflexivity and subsequent performance.
4.1 Frequencies of
behaviours at the team level of analysis
Figures 2, 3, and 4 depict
means and standard deviations of the reflective categories
across time during action and feedback phases. It can be seen
that evaluating is the most frequent reflective behaviour.
Looking for alternatives during action at Time 1 appears very
scarce. During feedback, it seems that looking for alternatives
is not a frequent practice. The same trend can be noted for
making decisions. It is more frequent during missions than after
feedback reception. It has to be noted that standard deviations
reflect important differences between teams. In sum, reflective
behaviours, when examined individually, tend to follow a similar
pattern: they appear more frequent during action while they are
low during feedback.
Figure 2. Means and
standard deviations of Evaluating
for each measurement time and phase (N = 32). (see pdf file)
Figure 3. Means and standard deviations of Looking for alternatives for each measurement time and phase (N = 32). (see pdf file)
Figure 4. Means and standard deviations of making decisions for each measurement time and phase (N = 32). (see pdf file)
4.2 Sequences of
evaluative behaviours
The coded reflective acts
described above are single communication behaviours. In the
present study, team reflexivity was conceptualised as a
collection of three behaviours. We explored whether teams
actually completed full ‘’reflective cycles’’ comprising all
three behaviours in a sequence. Since there has been no
empirical work demonstrating the necessity of all three
behaviours we also considered the most basic behavioural
patterns consisting of two subsequent reflective behaviours. As
can be seen in Table 2, most reflective communication across
teams can be summarised by two main two-behaviour sequences:
sequences starting with evaluating and looking for alternatives
and ending with clear decisions about a different way to handle
the task. In these sequences, teams “skipped” the evaluation or
search for alternatives phases. While sequences of looking for
alternatives followed by decisions seemed to grow over time
during action, except for the last mission, the frequencies of
the sequences starting with an evaluative comment and ending
with a decision stayed relatively stable over time, except for a
drop at Time 2. During feedback, the same trend than for
individual behaviours is observed; sequences are very scarce.
Importantly, full cycles were almost never completed, suggesting
that teams were not naturally systematic in their reflective
process. The absence of reflective cycles does not allow us to
further investigate their change over time and relatedness to
team performance. Still, how individual reflective behaviours
evolve over time and are related to team performance is of
importance to map 1) how teams naturally respond to feedback and
2) whether some behaviours appear more important than others in
the feedback process.
Table 2
Frequencies of Sequences of
Reflective Behaviours at each Wave of Data (N = 32)
Sequences |
Time 1 |
Time 2 |
Time 3 |
Time
4 |
|||
|
|
|
|
|
|
||
|
Action |
Feedback
|
Action |
Feedback |
Action |
Feedback |
Action |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
B1-B2 |
2 |
2 |
3 |
0 |
15 |
1 |
9 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
B1-B3 |
30 |
14 |
15 |
7 |
29 |
0 |
29 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
B2-B3 |
2 |
2 |
18 |
0 |
30 |
3 |
23 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
B1-B2-B3 |
0 |
0 |
1 |
0 |
2 |
0 |
0 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Notes. B1 = Evaluating,
B2 = Looking for alternatives, B3 = Making decisions.
4.3 Does team reflexivity
change over time?
To test for significant
changes of team reflexivity behaviours (considered individually)
over time, we computed repeated-measures analyses of variance
with a Greenhouse-Geisser correction (as sphericity was violated
for all behaviours) and with the four times each behaviour was
measured as a within-team factor. Pairwise comparisons with
Bonferroni corrections controlling for inflation of Type I error
were also computed. Evaluating during action changed
significantly from Time 1 to Time 4, F (2.26, 65.67) = 5.04, p =
.05, with pairwise comparisons showing that evaluating at Time 4
was significantly more frequent than evaluating at Time 2 (p =
.019). In contrast, evaluating during feedback did not change
over time. Regarding looking for alternatives during action
there was also an overall significant difference between the
means at the different time points, F (2.24, 64.84) = 5.76, p =
.004. A pairwise comparison confirmed a difference between Time
1 and Time 2 (p = .034), Time 1 and Time 3 (p = .000), and Time
1 and Time 4 (p = .0006), signifying an increase of the
behaviour over missions. Looking for alternatives during
feedback, making decisions during action and during feedback did
not change significantly over time with a Bonferroni correction.
With Tukey’s test, making decisions during action at Time 4 was
significantly higher than at Time 3 (p = .043) and Time 2 (p =
.045). Finally, while overall reflexivity across feedback did
not change significantly over time, the mean scores for overall
reflexivity across missions (i.e., aggregated behaviours) were
significantly different, F (2.33, 67.56) = 5.46 p = .004.
Specifically, reflexivity during mission at Time 4 was higher
than reflexivity during mission at Time 1 (p = .027) and Time 2
(p = .035). It is worthwhile noting that if the less
conservative Tukey post hoc test is used, the score in mission 3
is higher than at Time 1 (p = .015) and Time 2 (p = .043).
4.4 Is team reflexivity
related to team performance?
Intercorrelations between
reflective behaviours and performance measures are presented per
time period in Table 3.
Table 3
Correlations between Coded Categories and Performance Measures at each Point in Time (N = 32) (see pdf file)
Note. *p < .05. ** p < .01. T1= Time 1; T2=Mission
2; T3=Mission3; T4=Mission 4. Only significant correlations are
indicated
4.4.1 The impact of prior
team performance on team reflexivity
At first glance, initial
errors seem to be beneficial to subsequent reflective
behaviours, while the trend that errors trigger reflection tends
to decline with time. Specifically, the number of errors teams
made during the first action phase was positively correlated
with numerous reflective behaviours and to all reflective
categories. The number of crashes teams initially faced followed
the same path. Teams did not seem to be discouraged by their
first experience with crash. For example, the initial number of
crashes was positively related to more decision-making
behaviours after it occurred, both during the feedback phase
following the “failure” (r = .39, p < .05) and during the
next action at Time 2 (r = .47, p < .01). In contrast, errors
committed at Times 2 and 3 were not related to higher subsequent
reflective behaviours, with the exception of errors at Time 2
appearing as a trigger for decision making during the transition
phase immediately following that mission (r = .43, p < .05).
Conversely, errors at Times 2 and 3 seem to hamper decision
making during action at Time 4 (respectively r = - .42, p <
.05 and r = - .43, p < .05), while the number of crashes at
Time 2 was related to less frequent evaluative behaviours during
feedback at Time 2 (r = -.45, p < .05) and during action at
Time 4 (r = - .42, p < .05). In sum, these first results seem
to suggest that initial failure acts as an eye opener to
evaluate what went wrong, look for alternative ways of
approaching the task, and make more decisions, while later, as
task becomes more complex, it might relate to less subsequent
reflection.
4.4.2 The impact of team
reflexivity on subsequent team performance
Concerning the impact of
team reflexivity on subsequent performance, the correlations
indicate a differential impact depending on the type of
behaviour considered. The extent to which teams evaluated their
performance and strategies during feedback at Time 3 was related
to lower number of crashes at Time 4 (r = -.45, p < .05).
Similarly, the extent to which teams engaged in evaluative
behaviours during feedback at Time 2 significantly related to
fewer errors at Time 3 (r = -.42, p < .05). What is
particularly noticeable is that these significant relationships
concern 1) evaluative behaviours and 2) periods of team
transition, suggesting a positive effect of processing feedback
on subsequent performance. Reflection during action does not
seem to be significantly related to subsequent performance when
bivariate correlations are computed.
In contrast, the extent to
which teams made decisions during action at Time 1 was related
to more crashes at Time 3 (r = .48 p < .01), suggesting that
making decisions during the initial moments of team development
on this novel task (with its specific characteristics) may
impede subsequent performance.
5.
Discussion
To uncover when giving
teams feedback about their performance creates an opportunity
for learning, it has been posited that it is important to
examine how teams actively process feedback and thus
collaboratively evaluate information about past activities and
derive alternative recommendations for next action. However,
research on this feedback processing has been scarce in the
learning sciences and organisational psychology (Gabelica et
al., 2012; London & Sessa, 2006; Phielix, et al., 2010). It
is shown that the specific activities teams perform to deal with
feedback and when these activities are related to performance
remain to be considered.
Following from these
perceived gaps, we conducted a study attempting to build upon
and extend research in this domain by 1) identifying actual
behaviours enabling feedback processing (i.e., team reflexivity)
and 2) providing a more fine-grained analysis of dynamic aspects
of team reflexivity in a context with systematic and explicit
feedback. While theoretical work seems to suggest team
reflexivity is an iterative three-step cycle that involves
evaluating performance and strategies, looking for alternatives,
and making decisions (e.g., Schippers et al., 2007; Yukawa,
2006), we do not know if to perform well, it is necessary to
follow this series of steps and if some steps are more dominant
and influential than others in relation to team performance. We
explored the development of team reflective behaviours and the
relation between timing of reflective behaviours and performance
during four performance episodes.
Following conclusions could
be drawn. Firstly, teams never completed full cycles of
evaluating, looking for alternatives, and making decisions. They
did, however, completed two behaviour-sequences starting with
evaluating or looking for alternatives and ending with a clear
decision. Moreover, these sequences were very scarce during
transition (i.e., interrupts during which team performance
feedback was delivered). While the reflective cycle is usually
described sequentially, teams seem to rarely follow a rigid
series of steps to deal with feedback. Instead, it seems like
they often skip steps or even go back through steps several
times.
When taken individually,
team reflective behaviours were overall more frequent and
increased over time during action, whereas team reflexivity
during transition was relatively less frequent and did not
change. Looking for alternatives was very scarce but also
increased over time during action only. There might be two
reasons for this growth of reflection-in-action. First, natural
reflection might arise as a response to an immediate learning
need when a team observes cues of ineffective behaviours or
experience misunderstanding or uncertainty (e.g., they get lost)
while completing the task. Additionally, this learning need
could have been triggered by the increasing complexity of the
task (i.e., more cues to understand and interpret). Second,
preceding feedback, which has been advanced as a way to
counteract the natural decline of team reflexivity (Schippers et
al., 2003), could also have had a delayed effect on next
reflection-during action. In the learning sciences, feedback has
been shown to impact subsequent learning (e.g., Mory, 2003). It
may be that teams only see the learning content of a feedback
when they have to deal with a similar situation. This high
reflexivity-in-action suggests that teams are more reactive than
proactive and adaptive to anticipated circumstances when faced
with higher complexity and workload. This is in line with a
common rationale some authors have been using to speculate on
the causes of a lack of actual reflection in teams (Arvaja,
Häkkinen, Eteläpelto, & Rasku-Puttonen, 2000; Morris &
Stew, 2007). They state that teams are more driven by their
results (i.e., producing and performing) than the learning they
can gain, especially when they are under pressure and despite
the obvious benefits of strategy development (Gurtner et al.,
2007; Karau & Kelly, 1992). A possible explanation of why
teams did not use transition phases to increase reflection in
the same way lies in teams’ tendency to set their goals and
strategies early and not to actively question them later
(Argote, 1989; Hackman & Wageman, 2005; Weingart, 1992).
Further, it may be that the concurrent cognitive demands of
collaborating on the complex task, making sense of the received
feedback, and trying to prepare for the next task were too high
for teams to make the most of their learning experience
(Kirschner et al., 2009; Rummel & Spada, 2009). Reflecting
is a challenging high-order activity (Jay & Johnson, 2001).
The concept of reflection assumes individuals have the capacity
to engage in self-examination and open-minded analysis of their
own knowledge. Additionally, even if team members can reflect in
solo-learning situations, they may not be able to coordinate and
co-reflect in a team, communicate their reflective thoughts, nor
agree on ways to address the task (Chan, 2012).
Secondly, we uncovered
patterns that transcended the straightforward question of
whether team reflexivity can change performance in teams given
explicit feedback. It seems that the key question is rather when
improvements occur. (Lajoie & Lu, 2012) As signified in
recent research on team reflexivity (e.g., Moreland &
MacMinn, 2010; Schippers et al, 2013), reflexivity was not
uniformly beneficial. We showed instances in which timing of
reflective behaviours determined its effect (positive, negative,
or neutral) on performance. First, early decision-making was
related to lower subsequent outcomes. These findings are in
accordance with previous studies on timing of decision-making
indicating that early decision making might prevent deep
processing and sharing of unevenly distributed information (van
Ginkel, et al., 2009; van Ginkel & van Knippenberg, 2008).
Second, after that first experience with their task, teams were
able to derive insights from past performance (depicted by
feedback) and correct misunderstandings that prevented effective
action (Tjosvold et al., 2004). However, only evaluative
behaviours performed during feedback phases were related to
later improved performance. For example, teams were able to
reduce their errors in the last high-workload task with more
preparation (i.e., evaluating during preceding feedback time).
As such, the effect of evaluative behaviours seems to be
contingent on the phase during which they are performed (during
transition rather than during action). This points a paradox:
though they could experience positive consequences of reflecting
during these time outs, teams did not increase reflection during
feedback over time. Third, reflection during action does not
seem to be significantly related to better subsequent
performance when bivariate correlations are computed. As such,
no empirical support could be found for Moreland and McMinn’s
(2010) proposition that reflecting during the task could be more
beneficial to teams due to the immediate possibility to adjust
to the situation. However, this (increasing) reflection during
action did not harm team performance either and showed that, in
general, teams remained connected despite the increasingly
higher workload of their task. Communication breakdowns could
have been expected. Previous team research on non-routine
working situations (e.g., Waller et al., 2004) has demonstrated
that low-performing teams become cognitively overwhelmed in case
of high workload and consequently tend to focus more on their
individual sub-task instead of collaborating (e.g., Salas,
Rosen, & King, 2007).
Finally, initial errors
appeared to be a driver of subsequent team reflexivity while
later errors mostly did not have this motivational role. These
preliminary findings open up the possibility that there might be
some time-specific effects of previous errors that are
determinant to trigger motivation to improve. Maybe the first
mistake does not really hurt while repeated errors would be more
detrimental to performance?
Importantly, these results
raise the question of the causality of the relation between team
reflexivity and performance (e.g., Janssen et al., 2010). These
analyses seem to rather show a more dynamic and retroactive
relation between past and future performance and team
reflexivity, raising the need for time-series-analyses. This is
however behind the scope of the present explorative study. This
suggests that the question “does prior performance trigger team
reflexivity or does prior team reflexivity generate better
performance” should rather be changed into: when do performance
and team reflexivity dynamically interact to trigger team
learning and better subsequent performance. As we found a
reversed effect as well, it could be formulated that initial
errors do matter and that teams have the capability to learn
from them under certain circumstances.
Taken together, these
findings underscore the importance of a careful evaluation of
how the team is doing and why during transition phases
corresponding to feedback reception. We could not empirically
test the effects of three-step reflective cycles since they did
not naturally occur but we provided evidence that teams using
the feedback opportunity to stop and analyse their performance
and strategies were able to translate information about
performance into corrective behaviours since their performance
got improved. These results are line with studies on the impact
of interrupts (e.g., Okhuysen & Waller, 2002) and theories
on feedback in teams that stipulate that feedback receivers’
involvement plays a critical role to explain feedback
effectiveness in teams (e.g., Gabelica et al., 2012).
6.
Limitations and future directions
Although we have specified
behaviours signifying team reflexivity, we have not explored
depth of processing (Volet, Summers, & Thurman, 2009). It is
likely that deeper reflection (e.g., reflective statements
conveying inferences) generates better insight into the feedback
content and use in subsequent tasks (Anseel et al., 2009).
Also, previous research has
acknowledged that developing strategies for action is important
and necessary but does not always ensure actual strategy
implementation (Gurtner et al., 2007; Marks et al., 2001; Tschan
et al., 2000). Our third reflective category (i.e., making
decisions) encompassed both strategy development (i.e., clear
decisions about new strategies and actions) and strategy
implementation (i.e., clear gesture or overt behaviour showing
the team acting upon a decision). Despite the fact that teams
were provided with feedback based on specific criteria,
improvement strategies defined by teams could still have been
too general or abstract to be directly put into action, or
coordination problems could have render the well-defined plans
unused. A further investigation of implementation strategies and
their quality seems warranted.
Overall, this empirical
study was designed to meet methodological requirements of a
rigorous design with high internal validity, based on its
temporal sequencing and the collection of objective performance
data. Capturing fundamental processes in a controlled
environment is a first step to better understand complex
phenomena. To that purpose, we used a flight simulation as a
research platform to simulate and control task and team
features. Simulations constitute an interesting environment that
offers standardised performance measures with possibilities of
controlling complexity, information overload, and cues available
from the environment (Mathieu, 2000). In a learning perspective,
they also allow learners to apply their knowledge and
understanding to a task and observe the effects of their
decisions in a reactive environment that offers real-time
feedback (Bronack, Riedl, & Tashner, 2006; Gredler, 2004).
Prior work on simulations has shown that they stimulate numerous
cognitive processes, such as higher-level reasoning or creative
thinking (Moreno & Mayer, 2005; Moreno, Mayer, Spires, &
Lester, 2001). As a trade-off, laboratory environments overlook
natural factors in real-world contexts that may mediate
learning. As such, the extent to which the results from our
controlled design can be generalised to real had-hoc teams has
to be considered with cautious. The artificial and temporal
nature of the team and the limited number of team members must
be acknowledged. Teams of more than two team members and/or
knowing each other before completing a team task might exhibit
more complex interaction patterns. In this regard, we
acknowledge that there has been a recent debate about whether
findings from research on dyads can be simply generalised to
larger teams (Moreland, 2010; Williams, 2010). We are aware that
certain aspects of team processes and dynamics (e.g., group
socialization) can hardly be grasped by the use of dyads and
that the addition of team members increases complexity of team
communication and coordination (Michinov & Michinov, 2009;
Noroozi et al. 2012). However, since research into the dynamic
aspects of co-reflection in teams is a relatively new area, the
present study looking into the timing of basic team behaviours
in the smallest form of teams provides a very good start for
further research. A replication study with triads and larger
groups is needed to corroborate our results and explore the
relationship between team size and successful team reflexivity.
Moreover, although novelty of the task was controlled, we did
not account for group-ability composition while research has
demonstrated its influence on the accuracy and quality of
explanations in teams (Webb et al., 1998). Finally, the use of
students is sometimes considered as a possible limitation.
Nevertheless, previous studies have established that little
difference holds between the use of students and professional
teams when using problem solving and decision-making scenarios
(Balijepally et al., 2009; Yoo & Alavi, 2001). Still,
further research exploring the effects of reflexivity with
explicit feedback with more team members and in different
settings will be needed to understand the complexity of how team
members with different expertise, knowledge, and possibly high
diversity deal with feedback that describes the aggregated group
effort towards a shared goal. Furthermore, future field studies
obviously need to consider critical contextual factors that
influence and constraint team behaviours (Kozlowski & Ilgen,
2006).
Another limitation of the
study is the relatively lower frequency of some reflective
behaviours (e.g., looking for alternatives). Similarly,
sequences of reflective behaviours did not occur very often,
limiting the possible analyses relating these to performance
improvements. This necessitates caution about drawing premature
conclusions and underlines the need for replication studies with
larger samples. Additionally, this limitation could be overcome
in future research by stimulating or training teams to become
reflexive (e.g., King, 1991) and comparing occurrences of
reflective behaviours and their relation with team performance
with a no- training condition. Another challenging issue is
motivational: we do not know why teams did not frequently
reflect after feedback. Motivational factors behind a lack of
reflection and receptivity to feedback should be investigated in
further research.
Finally, the relation
between team performance (signified by accurate and specific
feedback) and team reflexivity should be analysed in
longitudinal designs. Additional measurement points of team
reflexivity spaced over time would provide a more fine-tuned
understanding of under which circumstances previous performance
has more impact on learning. Feedback loops in which previous
performance acts as an input for determining subsequent
processes and performance have been recently forwarded as
relevant models to understand team dynamics (llgen et al.,
2005).
7.
Practical implications
The present study suggests
that to be effective, feedback requires high levels of cognitive
engagement from learning teams. However, discussion of and
reflection on underlying reasons for success or failure,
alternatives, and improvement strategies does not seem to happen
spontaneously in teams, which in turn brings out the need to
provide them with appropriate external support. This has
potential implications for education. First, teachers should
uncover whether a lack of engagement in thoughtful analysis of
team experience is due to a lack of ability to perform shared
reflection (i.e., availability deficiency) or a lack of
execution of available skills (i.e., production deficiency). If
students know how to reflect as a team, prompts (i.e., scenarios
indicating how learners should interact) designed to induce
inferences and deep-oriented processing of the content of the
feedback appear an appropriate intervention to enhance teams’
motivation to engage in team reflexivity and to elicit learning
strategies which teams would not naturally demonstrate (Veenman
& Elshout, 1999). If students have not yet acquired the
skills needed to perform shared reflection, teachers may first
model and organise repeated practice with reflective cycles and
provide more guidance and structure in their prompts (King,
2007). However, it may be that when students have attained a
high level of reflective skills, prompts need to be withdrawn or
faded-out to facilitate internalization (Dillenbourg &
Tchounikine, 2007). Second, teachers should plan time and space
for feedback, in which errors are considered as learning
opportunities, and provide tools enabling students to actually
perform reflective activities on feedback (Gan & Hattie,
2014).
Keypoints
Acknowledgements
The authors are very
grateful to the two coders of the present study, Claudia
Baudewijns and Lubomira Nikolova.
References
Anseel, F., Lievens, F., & Schollaert, E. (2009). Reflection as a strategy to enhance task performance after feedback. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110, 23-35. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.05.003
Adcroft, A.
(2011). The mythology of feedback. Higher Education Research and Development 30(4),
405-419. doi:10.1080/07294360.2010.526096
Argote, L.
(1989). To centralize or not to centralize: the effects of
uncertainty and threat on group structure and performance. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 43, 58–74.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Arvaja, M.,
Häkkinen, P., Eteläpelto, A., & Rasku-Puttonen, H. (2000).
Collaborative processes during report writing of a science
learning project: The nature of discourse as a function of task
requirements. European
Journal of Psychology of Education, 15(4), 455-466.
doi:10.1007/BF03172987
Austin, J.,
Kessler, M. L., Riccobono, J. E., & Bailey, J. S. (1996).
Using feedback and reinforcement to improve the performance and
safety of a roofing crew. Journal
of Organizational Behavior Management, 16(2), 49-75.
doi:10.1300/J075v16n02_04
Ballard, D. I.,
Tschan, F., & Waller, M. J. (2008). All in the timing:
Considering time at multiple stages of group research. Small Group Research, 39,
328–351. doi:10.1177/1046496408317036
Balijepally, V.,
Mahapatra, R., Nerur, S., & Price, K. H. (2009). Are two
Heads better than one for Software Development? The Productivity
Paradox of Pair Programming. MIS Quarterly, 33(1),
91-118.
Barron, B.
(2000). Achieving coordination in collaborative problem-solving
groups. Journal of the
Learning Sciences, 9, 403–436.
doi:10.1207/S15327809JLS0904_2
Boud, D., Keogh,
R., & Walker, D. (1985). Promoting reflection in learning: A
model. In D. Boud, R. Keogh, & D. Walker (Eds.), Reflection:
Turning experience into learning (pp. 18-40). London:
Kogan Page.
Boud, D., &
E. Molloy. (2013). Rethinking Models of Feedback for Learning:
The Challenge of Design. Assessment
& Evaluation in Higher Education, 38(6), 698-712.
doi:10.1080/02602938.2012.691462
Bowers, C. A.,
Salas, E., Prince, C., & Brannick, M. T. (1992). Games teams
play: A methodology for investigating team coordination and
performance. Behavior
Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 24, 503-506. doi:10.3758/BF03203594
Bronack, S.,
Riedl, R., & Tashner, J. (2006) Learning in the zone: A
social constructivist framework for distance education in a 3D
virtual world. Interactive
Learning Environments, 14(3), 219-232.
doi:10.1080/10494820600909157
Chan, C. K. K.
(2012). Co-regulation of learning in computer-supported
collaborative learning environments: a discussion, Metacognition and Learning,
7(1), 63-73. doi:10.1007/s11409-012-9086-z
Chi,
M.
T. H. (1997). Quantifying qualitative analysis of verbal data: A
practical guide. The
Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6, 271-315. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls0603_1
Decuyper,
S.,
Dochy, F., & Van den Bossche, P. (2010). Grasping the
dynamic complexity of team learning: An integrative model for
effective team learning in organizations. Educational Research
Review, 5, 111-133. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2010.02.002
De
Wever, B., Schellens, T., Valcke, M., & Van Keer, H. (2006).
Content analysis schemes to analyze transcripts of online
asynchronous discussion groups: A review. Computers & Education,
46(1), 6–28. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Dewey,
J.
(1910/1997). How we
think. Mineola, New York: Dover.
Dillenbourg,
P.
(1999). Collaborative
Learning: Cognitive and Computational Approaches. Advances in
Learning and Instruction Series. New York, NY: Elsevier
Science, Inc.
Dillenbourg,
P.,
Baker, M., Blaye, A, & O’Malley, C. (1996). The evolution of
research on collaborative learning. In E. Spada & P.
Reiman (Eds), Learning in
Humans and Machine: Towards an interdisciplinary learning
science (pp. 189-211). Oxford: Elsevier.
Dillenbourg,
P.,
& Tchounikine, P. (2007). Flexibility in macro-scripts for
computer-supported collaborative learning. Journal of Computer
Assisted Learning, 23(1), 1–13.
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2007.00191.x
Dochy,
F.,
Gijbels, D., Raes, E., & Kyndt, E. (2014, forthcoming). Team
learning: Research in education and professional contexts. In S.
Billett, C. Harteis, and H. Gruber (eds.), International Handbook of
Research in Professional Practice-based Learning.
Springer.
Duijnhouwer,
H.,
Prins, F. J., , Stokking, K. M. (2012). Feedback providing
improvement strategies and reflection on feedback use: Effects
on students’ writing motivation, process, and performance. Learning and Instruction,
22(4), 171-184. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.10.003
Edmondson,
A.
C. (1999). Psychological safety and learning behaviour in work
teams. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 44(2), 350–383. doi:10.2307/2666999
Edmondson, A. C, Bohmer, R. M, &
Pisano, G. P. (2001). Disrupted Routines: Team Learning and New
Technology Implementation in Hospitals. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 46, 685-716. doi:10.2307/3094828
Edmondson, A. C., Dillon, J., &
Roloff, K. S., (2007). Three perspectives on team learning
outcome improvement, task mastery, and group process. The Academy of Management
Annals, 1, 269-314. doi:10.1080/078559811
Entin, E. E., & Serfaty, D. (1999).
Adaptive team coordination. Human Factors, 41(2), 312-325.
doi:10.1518/001872099779591196
Eriksen, J., & Dyer, L. (2004).
Right from the start: Exploring the effects of early team events
on subsequent project team development and performance. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 49, 438–471. doi:10.2307/4131442
Eva, K. W., Armson, H., Holmboe, E.,
Lockyer, J., Loney, E., Mann, K., et al. (2012). Factors
influencing responsiveness to feedback: On the interplay between
fear, confidence, and reasoning processes. Advances in Health
Sciences Education: Theory and Practice, 17(1), 15–26.
doi:10.1007/s10459-011-9290-7
Gabelica, C., Van den Bossche, P.,
Segers, M., & Gijselaers, W. (2012). Feedback, a Powerful
Lever in teams: a Review. Educational
Research
Review, 7(2), 123-144. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Gan, M. J. S., & Hattie, J. (2014).
Prompting secondary students’ use of criteria, feedback
specificity and feedback levels during an investigative task. Instructional Science.
doi:10.1007/s11251-014-9319-4.
Gersick, C. J. G. (1989). Marking time:
Predictable transition in task groups. Academy of Management
Journal, 32, 274-309. doi:10.2307/256363
Goodman,
J.
S., Wood, R. E., & Hendrickx, M. (2004). Feedback
specificity, exploration, and learning. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 89(2),
248-262. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.89.2.248
Gredler, M. E. (2004). Games and
simulations and their relationships to learning. In D. H.
Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook
of research for educational communications and technology
(2nd ed., pp. 571-82). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gurtner, A., Tschan, F., Semmer, N. K.,
& Nagele, C. (2007). Getting groups to develop good
strategies: Effects of reflexivity interventions on team
process, team performance, and shared mental models. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 102, 127–142.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Hackman, J. R., & Wageman, R.
(2005). A theory of team coaching. Academy of Management
Review, 30, 269-287. doi:10.5465/AMR.2005.16387885
Hattie, J. (2013). Calibration and
confidence: Where to next? Learning and Instruction,
24, 62-66. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007).
The power of feedback. Review
of Educational Research, 77(1), 81-112.
doi:10.3102/003465430298487
Hoegl, M., & Parboteeah, K. P.
(2006). Team goal commitment in innovative projects. International
Journal of Innovation Management, 10(3), 299–324.
doi:10.1142/S136391960600151X.
Ilgen, D. R., Hollenbeck, J. R., Johnson, M., & Jundt, D. (2005). Teams in organizations: From I-P-O models to IMOI models. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 517-543. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070250
Jay, J. K., & Johnson, K. L.
(2001). Capturing complexity: A typology of reflective practice
for teacher education. Teaching
and
Teacher Education, 18(1), 73-85. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Janssen, J., Kirschner, F., Erkens, G.,
Kirschner, P. A., & Paas, F. (2010). Making the black box of
collaborative learning transparent: Combining process-oriented
and cognitive load approaches. Educational Psychology
Review, 22,139-154. doi:10.1007/s10648-010-9131-x
Järvelä, S., Volet, S., &
Järvenoja, H. (2010). Research on motivation in collaborative
learning: Moving beyond the cognitive-situative divide and
combining individual and social processes. Educational
Psychologist, 45, 15–27. doi:10.1080/00461520903433539
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T.
(1992). Key to effective cooperation. In R. Hertz-Lazarowitz,
& N. Miller (Eds.), Interaction
in cooperative groups. The theoretical anatomy of
group learning (pp. 174–199). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T.
(1993). What we know about cooperative learning at the college
level. Cooperative
Learning, 13(3), 17–18.
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., &
Stanne, M. E. (2000). Cooperative Learning Methods: A
meta-analysis. University of Minnesota, Minneapolis: Cooperative
Learning Center. Retrieved from:
http://www.co-operation.org/pages/cl-methods.html.
Jung, D. I., & Sosik, J. J. (2003).
Group potency and collective efficacy: Examining their
predictive validity, level of analysis, and effects of
performance feedback on future group performance. Group & Organization
Management, 28,
366-391. doi:10.1177/1059601102250821
Kaplan, S., LaPort, K., & Waller,
M. J. (2013). The role of positive affect in team effectiveness
during crises. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 34(4), 427-580.
doi:10.1002/job.1817
Kapur, M., Voiklis, J., & Kinzer,
C. K. (2008). Sensitivities to early exchange in synchronous
computer-supported collaborative learning (CSCL) groups. Computers & Education,
51(1), 54-66. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2007.04.007
Karau, S. J., & Kelly, J. R.
(1992). The effects of time scarcity and time abundance on group
performance quality and interaction process. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 28,
542–571. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Killion, J., & Todnem, G. (1991). A
process for personal theory building. Educational Leadership, 48(6),
14-16.
King, A. (2007). Scripting
collaborative learning processes: A cognitive perspective. In F.
Fischer, I. Kollar, H. Mandl, & J. M. Haake (Vol. Eds.),
Computer-supported collaborative learning series: Vol. 6.
Scripting computer-supported collaborative learning (pp. 13–37).
New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-36949-5.
King, A. (1991). Effects of training in
strategic questioning on children’s problem-solving performance.
Journal of Educational
Psychology, 83(3), 307–317.
Kirschner, P. A. (2009). Epistemology
or pedagogy, that is the question. In: S. Tobias & T.M.
Duffy (eds.), Constructivist
theory applied to instruction: Success or failure? (pp.
144-157). New York: Routledge.
Kirschner, F., Paas, F., &
Kirschner, P. A. (2008). A cognitive load approach to
collaborative learning: United brains for complex tasks. Educational Psychology
Review, 21,
31-42. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.008
Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996).
The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A
historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback
intervention theory. Psychological
Bulletin, 119, 254-284. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.73.1.87
Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Ilgen, D. R.
(2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of work groups and teams. Psychological Science in
the Public Interest,
7, 77–124. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00030.x
Kramarski, B. (2004). Making sense of
graphs: Does metacognitive instruction make a difference on
students’ mathematical conceptions and alternative conceptions.
Learning and Instruction,
14, 593-619. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Lajoie, S. P., & Lu, J. (2012).
Supporting collaboration with technology: Does shared cognition
lead to co-regulation in medicine. Metacognition and Learning,
7, 45-46. doi:10.1007/s11409-011-9077-5
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G.
(1977). The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical
Data, Biometrics, 33,
159-174.
Leicht, R. M., Hunter, S. T., Saluja,
C., & Messner, J. I. (2010). Implementing observational
research methods to study team performance in construction
management. Journal of
Construction Engineering and Management, 136(1), 76–86. doi:10.1061/(ASCE)CO.1943-7862.0000080
Lewis, K., Belliveau, M., Herndon, B,
& Keller, J. (2007). Group cognition, membership change, and
performance: Investigating the benefits and detriments of
collective knowledge. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103(2), 159-178.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
London, M. (2003). Job feedback: Giving,
seeking, and using feedback for performance improvement
(2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
London, M., & Sessa, V. I. (2006).
Group feedback for continuous learning. Human Ressource Development
Review, 5(3),
1-27. doi:10.1177/1534484306290226
Marks, M. A., Mathieu, J. E., &
Zaccaro, S. J. (2001). A temporally based framework and taxonomy
of team processes. Academy
of Management Review, 26,356–376. doi: 10.2307/259182
Mathieu, J. E., & Rapp, T. L.
(2009). Laying the foundation for successful team performance
trajectories: The role of team charters and performance
strategies. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 94, 90 –103. doi:10.1037/a0013257
McCarthy, A., & Garavan, T. (2008).
Team learning and metacognition: A neglected area of HRD
research and practice. Advances
in Developing Human Resources, 10, 509-524. doi:10.1177/1523422308320496
McGrath, J. E. (1993). The JEMCO
workshop: Description of a longitudinal study, Small Group Research,
24, 285–306.
doi:10.1177/1046496493243002
Michinov, N., & Michinov, E.
(2009). Investigating the relationship between transactive
memory and performance in collaborative learning. Learning and Instruction,
19(1), 43–54. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.01.003
Moreland, R. L.,
& McMinn, J. G. (2010). Group reflexivity and performance.
In S. R. Thye & E. Lawler (Eds.), Advances in group processes
(Vol. 27, pp. 63-95). Bingley, UK: Emerald Press.
Moreno, R.,
Mayer, R. E., Spires, H. A., & Lester, J. C. (2001). The
case for social agency in computer-based teaching. Do students
learn more deeply when they interact with animated pedagogical
agents? Cognition and
Instruction, 19(2), 177-213.
doi:10.1207/S1532690XCI1902_02
Mory, E. H.
(2003). Feedback research revisited. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of research on
educational communications and technology (pp. 745-783).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Morris, J., &
Stew, G. (2007). Collaborative reflection: how far do 2:1 models
of learning in the practice setting promote peer reflection, Reflective Practice, 8(3),
419-432. doi:10.1080/14623940701425220
Mulder, R. H.,
& Ellinger, A. D. (2013). Perceptions of quality of feedback
in organizations: Characteristics, determinants, outcomes of
feedback, and possibilities for improvement: Introduction to a
special issue. European Journal
of Training and Development, 37(1), 4-23.
doi:10.1108/03090591311293266
Mulder, R. H.
(2013). Exploring feedback incidents, their characteristics and
the informal learning activities that emanate from them. European Journal of
Training and Development, 37(1), 49-71.
Müller, A., Herbig, B. , &
Petrovic, K. (2009). The explication of implicit team knowledge
and its supporting effect on team processes and technical
innovations. An action regulation perspective on team
reflexivity. Small Group
Research, 40, 28-51. doi:10.1177/1046496408326574
Mullins, D., Rummel, N., & Spada,
H. (2011). Are two heads always better than one? Differential
effects of collaboration on students' computer-supported
learning in mathematics. International
Journal
of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning, 6(3),
421-443. doi:10.1007/s11412-011-9122-z
Noroozi, O., Weinberger, Biemans, H. J.
A., Mulder, M., & Chizari, M. (2012). Argumentation-based
computer supported collaborative learning (ABCSCL). A systematic
review and synthesis of fifteen years of research. Educational Research
Review, 7(2), 79–106. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.03.001
Okhuysen,
G.
A., & Eisenhardt, K. M. (2002). Integrating knowledge in
groups: How simple formal interventions help. Organization Science, 13,
370-386. doi:10.1287/orsc.13.4.370.2947
Okhuysen,
G.,
& Waller, M. J. (2002). Focusing on midpoint transitions: An
analysis of boundary conditions. Academy of Management
Journal, 45, 1056-1065. doi:10.2307/3069330
Phielix,
C.,
Prins, F. J., & Kirschner, P. A. (2010). Awareness of group
performance in a CSCL environment: Effects of peer feedback and
reflection. Computers in
Human Behavior, 26, 151-161. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Phielix,
C., Prins, F. J., Kirschner, P. A., Erkens, G., & Jaspers,
J. G. M. (2011). Group awareness of social and cognitive
performance in a CSCL environment: Effects of a peer feedback
and reflection tool. Computers
in Human Behavior, 27(3), 1087-1102.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Prins, F. J.,
Sluijsmans, D. M. A., & Kirschner, P. A. (2006). Feedback
for general practitioners in training: quality, styles, and
preferences. Advances in
Health Sciences Education, 11, 289-303.
doi:10.1007/s10459-005-3250-z
Prinsen, F. R.,
Terwel, J., Zijlstra, B. J. H., & Volman, M. M. L. (2013).
The effects of guided elaboration in a CSCL programme on the
learning outcomes of primary school students from Dutch and
immigrant families. Educational
Research and Evaluation, 19(1), 39–57.
doi:10.1080/13803611.2012.744694
Reimann, P.
(2007). Time is precious: Why process analysis is essential for
CSCL (and can also help to bridge between experimental and
descriptive methods). In C. Chinn, G. Erkens & S.
Puntambekar (Eds.), Mice,
minds, and society. Proceedings of the computer-supported
collaborative learning conference (CSCL 2007) (pp.
598–607). New Brunswick, NJ: International Society of the
Learning Sciences.
Rourke, L., &
Anderson, T. (2004). Validity in quantitative content analysis.
Education Technology
Research and Development, 52(1), 5-18.
doi:10.1007/BF02504769
Rourke, L.,
Anderson, T., Garrison, D. R., & Archer, W. (2001).
Methodological issues in the content analysis of computer
conference transcripts.
International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education,
12, 8-22
Rulke, D. L.,
& Rau, D. (2000). Investigating the encoding process of
transactive memory development in group training. Group & Organization
Management, 25, 373-396.
Rummel, N.,
Mullins, D., & Spada, H. (2012). Scripted collaborative
learning with the Cognitive Tutor Algebra. International Journal of
Computer Supported Collaborative Learning, 7(2), 307-339.
doi:10.1007/s11412-012-9146-z
Rummel, N., &
Spada, H. (2005). Learning to Collaborate: An Instructional
Approach to Promoting Collaborative Problem Solving in
Computer-Mediated Settings. Journal of the Learning
Sciences, 14(2), 201-241. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1402_2
Rummel, N.,
Spada, H., & Hauser, S. (2009). Learning to collaborate from
being scripted or from observing a model. International Journal of
Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 4(1), 69-92.
doi: 10.1007/s11412-008-9054-4
Salas, E.
Dickinson, T. L., Converse, S. A., & Tannenbaum, S. I.
(1992). Toward an understanding of team performance and
training. In R. W. Swezey & E. Salas (Eds.), Teams:
Their training and performance (pp. 3-29). Norwood, NJ:
Ablex.
Salas, E., Rosen,
M. A., & King, H. (2007). Managing teams managing crises:
Principles of teamwork to improve patient safety in the
emergency room and beyond. Theoretical Issues in
Ergonomics Science, 8(5), 381-394. doi:10.1080/14639220701317764
Salas, E., Stagl,
K. C., & Burke, C. S. (2004). 25 years of team effectiveness
in organizations: Research themes and emerging needs. In C. L.
Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of
industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 47–91). New
York: Wiley.
Savelsbergh, C.,
Heijden, B. I. J. M. van der, & Poell, R. F. (2009). The
development and empirical validation of a multi-dimensional
measurement instrument for team learning behaviors. Small
Group Research, 40(5), 578-607. doi:10.1177/1046496409340055
Schippers, M. C.,
Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2007). Reflexivity in
teams: A measure and correlates.
Applied
Psychology-An International Review, 56(2), 189-211. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00250.x
Schippers, M. C.,
Den Hartog, D. N., Koopman, P. L., & Wienk, J. A. (2003).
Diversity and team outcomes: The moderating effects of outcome
interdependence and group longevity and the mediating effect
of reflexivity. Journal
of Organizational Behavior, 24, 779-802. doi:10.1002/job.220
Schippers, M. C.,
Den Hartog, D. N., Koopman , P. L., & van Knippenberg, D.
(2008). The role of transformational leadership in enhancing
team reflexivity. Human Relations, 61, 1593-1616. doi:10.1177/0018726708096639
Schippers, M. C., Homan, A. C., &
van Knippenberg, D. (2013). To reflect or not to reflect: Prior team
performance as a boundary condition of the effects of
reflexivity on learning and final team performance. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 34(1), 6-23. doi:10.1002/job.1784
Schön, D. (1983). The reflective
practitioner: How professionals think in action. New
York: Basic.
Scott-Young, C., &
Samson, D. (2009). How team efficacy beliefs impact project
performance: An empirical investigation of team potency in
capital projects in the process industries. World Academy of Science,
Engineering and Technology, 3, 6-28. doi:10.1.1.308.8306
Seibert, K. W.
(1999). Reflection-in-action: Tools for cultivating on-the-job
learning conditions. Organizational
Dynamics, 27, 54-65. doi:10.1016/S0090-2616(99)90021-9
Soller, A.,
Monés, A. M., Jermann, P., & Mühlenbrock, M. (2005). From
Mirroring to Guiding: A Review of State of the Art Technology
for Supporting Collaborative Learning. International Journal on
Artificial Intelligence in Education, 15(5), 261– 290. doi:10.1.1.26.2186
Sims, D. E.,
Salas, E., & Burke, C. S. (2005). Promoting effective team
performance through training. In S. A. Wheelan (Ed.), The Handbook of Group
Research and Practice (pp. 407-425). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Shute, V. J.
(2008). Focus on formative feedback. Review of Educational
Research, 78, 153-189. doi:10.3102/0034654307313795
Tschan, F.,
Semmer, N. K., Nägele, C., & Gurtner, A. (2000). Task
adaptive behavior and performance in groups. Group Processes and
Intergroup Relations, 3(4), 367–386. doi:10.1177/1368430200003004003
Tjosvold, D.,
Tang, M. M. L., & West, M. (2004). Reflexivity for team
innovation in China: The contribution of goal interdependence. Group & Organization
Management, 29(5), 540–559. doi:10.1177/1059601103254911
van Ginkel, W.,
Tindale, R. S., & van Knippenberg, D. (2009). Team
reflexivity, development of shared task representations, and the
use of distributed information in group decision making. Group Dynamics, 13,
265-280. doi:10.1037/a0016045
van Ginkel, W. P., & van
Knippenberg, D. (2009). Knowledge about the distribution of information
and group decision making: When and why does it work? Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 108(2), 218-229. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.10.003
Van der Haar, S., Segers,
M. R. S, & Jehn, K. A. (2013). Towards a contextualized
model of team learning processes and outcomes. Educational Research
Review, 10, 1-12. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2013.04.001
Van der Pol, J., Van den
Berg, I., Admiraal, W. F., & Simons, P. R. J. (2008). The
nature, reception, and use of online peer feedback in higher
education. Computers
& Education, 51, 1804-1817.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Veenman, M. V. J., &
Elshout, J. J. (1999) Changes in the relation between
cognitive and metacognitive skills during the acquisition of
expertise. European
Journal of Psychology of Education, 14, 509-523.
doi:10.1007/BF03172976
Veestraeten, M., Kyndt, E.,
& Dochy, F. (2014). Investigating Team Learning in a
Military Context. Vocation
and Learning, 7(1), 75-100.
doi:10.1007/s12186-013-9107-3
Villado, A. J., &
Arthur, W. Jr. (2013). The comparative effect of subjective
and objective after-action reviews on team performance on a
complex task. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 514-528. doi:10.1037/a0031510
Visschers-Pleijers,
A. J. S. F., Dolmans, D. H. J. M., De Leng, B. A., Wolfhagen, H.
A. P., & Van der Vleuten, C. P. M. (2006). Analysis of
verbal interactions in tutorial groups: A process study. Medical Education, 40,
129-137. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2929.2005.02368.x
Volet, S., Summers, M.,
& Thurman, J. (2009). High-level co-regulation in
collaborative learning: how does it emerge and how is it
sustained? Learning and
Instruction, 19, 128–143. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031
Waller, M. J. (1999). The timing of
adaptive group responses to nonroutine events. Academy of Management
Journal, 42, 127–137. doi:10.2307/257088
Waller, M. J., Gupta, N., &
Giambatista, R. C. (2004). Effects of adaptive behaviors and
shared mental models on control crew performance. Management Science, 50,
1534–1544. doi:10.1287/mnsc.1040.0210
Weingart, L. R. (1992). Impact of group
goals, task component complexity, effort, and planning on group
performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 77(5), 682–693. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.77.5.682
West, M. A.
(Ed.) (1996). Handbook of
work group psychology, Chichester: John Wiley & Sons,
Ltd.
West, M. A.
(2000). Reflexivity, revolution and innovation in work teams. In
M. M. Beyerlein, D.A. Johnson & S.T. Beyerlein (Eds), Product development teams,
Vol. 5. (pp. 1-29). Stamford, CT: JAI Press.
West, M. A.,
Garrod, S., & Carletta, J. (1997). Group decision-making and
effectiveness: unexplored boundaries. In C.L. Cooper & S.E.
Jackson (Eds.), Creating
tomorrow’s organizations: A handbook for future research in
organizational behavior (pp. 293- 316). Chichester: John
Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Williams, K. D.
(2010). Dyads can be groups (and often are). Small Group Research,
41, 268-274. doi:10.1177/1046496409358619
Wills, K. V., & Clerkin, T. A. (2009).
Incorporating reflective practice into team simulation projects
for improved learning outcomes. Business Communication
Quarterly, 72(2),
221 227. doi:10.1177/1080569909334559
Yang, M., & Carless, D. (2013). The
feedback triangle and the enhancement of dialogic feedback
processes. Teaching in
Higher Education. 18(3), 285-297.
doi:10.1080/13562517.2012.719154
Yoo, Y., & Alavi, M. (2001) Media
and group cohesion: Relative influences on social presence, task
participation, and group consensus, MIS Quarterly, 25(3),
371-390. doi:10.2307/3250922
Yukawa, J. (2006). Co-reflection in online learning: Collaborative critical thinking as narrative. Journal of Computer-supported Collaborative Learning, 1, 203–228. doi:10.1007/s11412-006-8994-9