Effectiveness of Self-Generation During Learning is Dependent on Individual Differences in Need for Cognition.
Main Article Content
Abstract
Self-generated information is better recognized and recalled than read information. This so-called generation effecthas been replicated several times for different types of stimulus material, different generation tasks, and retention intervals. The present study investigated the impact of individual differences in learners’ disposition to engage in effortful cognitive activities (need for cognition, NFC) on the effectiveness of self-generation during learning. Learners low in NFC usually avoid getting engaged in cognitively demanding activities. However, if these learners are explicitly instructed to use elaborate learning strategies such as self-generation, they should benefit more from such strategies than learners high in NFC, because self-generation stimulates cognitive processes that learners low in NFC usually tend not to engage in spontaneously. Using a classical word-generation paradigm, we not only replicated the generation effect in free and cued recall but showed that the magnitude of the generation effect increased with decreasing NFC in cued recall. Results are consistent with our assumption that learners higher in NFC engage in elaborate processing even without explicit instruction, whereas learners lower in NFC usually avoid cognitively demanding activities. These learners need cognitively demanding tasks that require them to switch from shallow to elaborate processing to improve learning. We conclude that self-generation is beneficial regardless of the NFC level, but our study extends the existing literature on the generation effect and on NFC by showing that self-generation can be particularly useful for balancing the learning disadvantage of students lower in NFC.
Article Details
FLR adopts the Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs Creative Common License (BY-NC-ND). That is, Copyright for articles published in this journal is retained by the authors with, however, first publication rights granted to the journal. By virtue of their appearance in this open access journal, articles are free to use, with proper attribution, in educational and other non-commercial settings.
References
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., & Sigmann, H. (2014). Lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 [Software]. R-package version 1.1-6. Retrieved May 1, 2014 from: http://cran.r-project.org/package=lme4
Bertrams, A., & Dickhäuser, O. (2009). High-school students' need for cognition, self-control capacity, and school achievement: Testing a mediation hypothesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(1), 135–138. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2008.06.005
Bertsch, S., Pesta, B. J., Wiscott, R., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007). The generation effect: A meta-analytic review. Memory & Cognition, 35(2), 201–210. doi:10.3758/BF03193441
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp.185–205). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher, R. W. Pew, L. M. Hough, & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 56–64). New York: Worth Publishers.
Bless, H., Wänke, M., Bohner, G., Fellhauer, R. F., & Schwarz, N. (1994). Need for Cognition: Eine Skala zur Erfassung von Engagement und Freude bei Denkaufgaben [Need for cognition: A scale measuring engagement and happiness in cognitive tasks]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 25, 147–154.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197–253. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 306–307. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13
Cacioppo, J T., Petty, R. E., Kao, C. F., & Rodriguez, R. (1986). Central and peripheral routes to persuasion: An individual difference perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(5), 1032–1043. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.5.1032
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Morris, K. J. (1983). Effects of need for cognition on message evaluation, recall, and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(4), 805–818. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.45.4.805
Cazan, A.-M., & Indreica, S. E. (2014). Need for cognition and approaches to learning among university students. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 127, 134–138. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.03.227
Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 354–380. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354
Coutinho, S. A. (2006). The relationship between the need for cognition, metacognition, and intellectual task performance. Educational Research and Reviews, 1(5), 162–164.
Coutinho, S. A., Wiemer-Hastings, K., Skowronski, J. J., & Britt, M. A. (2005). Metacognition, need for cognition and use of explanations during ongoing learning and problem solving. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(4), 321–337. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.06.001
Dalbert, C. (1999). The world is more just for me than generally: About the personal belief in a just world scale’s validity. Social Justice Research, 12(2), 79–98. doi:10.1023/A:1022091609047
Dickhäuser, O., Schöne, C., Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2002). Die Skalen zum akademischen Selbstkonzept: Konstruktion und Überprüfung eines neuen Instrumentes [The academic self-concept scales: Construction and evaluation of a new instrument]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 23(4), 393–405. doi:10.1024//0170-1789.23.4.393
Dixon, P. (2008). Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 447–456. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.004
Doctorow, M., Wittrock, M. C., & Marks, C. (1978). Generative processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(2), 109–118. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.70.2.109
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58. doi:10.1177/1529100612453266
Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Owen, P. D., & Coté, N. C. (1990). Encoding and recall of texts: The importance of material appropriate processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(5), 566–581. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90052-2
Fiedler, K., Lachnit, H., Fay, D., & Krug, C. (1992). Mobilization of cognitive resources and the generation effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A, 45(1), 149–171. doi:10.1080/14640749208401320
Fleischhauer, M., Miller, R., Enge, S., & Albrecht, T. (2014). Need for cognition relates to low-level visual performance in a metacontrast masking paradigm. Journal of Research in Personality, 48, 45–50. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.09.007
Gardiner, J. M., & Rowley, J. M. C. (1984). A generation effect with numbers rather than words. Memory & Cognition, 12(5), 443–445. doi:10.3758/BF03198305
Graf, P. (1980). Two consequences of generating: Increased inter- and intraword organization of sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(3), 316–327. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90248-0
Graf, P. (1981). Reading and generating normal and transformed sentences. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 35(4), 293–308. doi:10.1037/h0081193
Grass, J., Strobel, A., & Strobel, A. (2017). Cognitive investments in academic success: The role of need for cognition at university. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 790. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00790
Heijne-Penninga, M., Kuks, J. B. M., Hofman, W. H. A., & Cohen-Schotanus, J. (2010). Influences of deep learning, need for cognition and preparation time on open- and closed-book test performance. Medical Education, 44(9), 884–891. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2923.2010.03732.x
Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97–110.
Jebb, A. T., Saef, R., Parrigon, S., & Woo, S. E. (2016). The need for cognition: Key concepts, assessment, and role in educational outcomes. In A. Lipnevich, F. Preckel, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Psychosocial skills and school systems in the twenty-first century: Theory, research, and applications. The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality (pp.115–132). New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-28606-8_5
Kardash, C. A. M., & Noel, L. K. (2000). How organizational signals, need for cognition, and verbal ability affect text recall and recognition. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(3), 317–331. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1011
Kirsh, D. (2006). Distributed cognition. A methodological note. Pragmatics and Cognition, 14(2), 249–262). doi:10.1075/pc.14.2.06kir
Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513–549. doi:10.1016/0364-0213(94)90007-8
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package). R-package version 2.0-6. Retrieved in June 2014 from: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.html
Leone, C., & Dalton, C. H. (1988). Some effects of the need for cognition on course grades. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67(1), 175–178. doi:10.2466/pms.1988.67.1.175
Luong, C., Strobel, A., Wollschläger, R., Greiff, S., Vainikainen, M.-P., & Preckel, F. (2017). Need for cognition in children and adolescents: Behavioral correlates and relations to academic achievement and potential. Learning and Individual Differences, 53, 103–113. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.019
Maglio, P. P. & Kirsh, D. (1996). Epistemic action increases with skill. In G. W. Cottrell (Ed.), Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 391–396). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McDaniel, M. A., & Butler, A. C. (2010). A contextual framework for understanding when difficulties are desirable. In A. S. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful Remembering and Successful Forgetting: A Festschrift in Honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 175-198). New York: Taylor and Francis. doi:10.4324/9780203842539
McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1989). Material-appropriate processing: A contextualist approach to reading and studying strategies. Educational Psychology Review, 1(2), 113–145. doi:10.1007/BF01326639
McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2005). Material appropriate difficulty: A framework for determining when difficulty is desirable for improving learning. In A. F. Healy (Ed.), Experimental cognitive psychology and its applications (pp. 73–85). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10895-006
McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., Dunay, P. K., & Cobb, R. E. (1986). Encoding difficulty and memory: Toward a unifying theory. Journal of Memory and Language, 25(6), 645–656. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(86)90041-0
McDaniel, M. A., Hines, R. J., & Guynn, M. J. (2002). When text difficulty benefits less-skilled readers. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(3), 544–561. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2819
McDaniel, M. A., & Waddill, P. J. (1990). Generation effects for context words: Implications for item-specific and multifactor theories. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(2), 201–211. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(90)90072-8
McDaniel, M. A., Waddill, P. J., & Einstein, G. O. (1988). A contextual account of the generation effect: A three-factor theory. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(5), 521–536. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(88)90023-X
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(5), 519–533. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9
Nair, K. U., & Ramnarayan, S. (2000). Individual differences in need for cognition and complex problem solving. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(3), 305–328. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1999.2274
Reinhard, M.-A (2010). Need for cognition and the process of lie detection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 961–971. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.06.002
Reinhard, M.-A., & Dickhäuser, O. (2009). Need for cognition, task difficulty, and the formation of performance expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1062–1076. doi:10.1037/a0014927
Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 353–387. doi:10.1037/a0026838
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249–255. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x
Sadowski, C. J., & Gülgöz, S. (1996). Elaborative processing mediates the relationship between need for cognition and academic performance. The Journal of Psychology, 130(3), 303–307. doi:10.1080/00223980.1996.9915011
Schweickert, R., McDaniel, M. A., & Riegler, G. (1994). Effects of generation on immediate memory span and delayed unexpected free recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A, 47(3), 781–804. doi:10.1080/14640749408401137
See, Y. H. M., Petty, R. E., & Evans, L. M. (2009). The impact of perceived message complexity and need for cognition on information processing and attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(5), 880–889. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.006
Slamecka, N. J., & Fevreiski, J. (1983). The generation effect when generation fails. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22(2), 153–163. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90112-3
Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 592–604. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.592
Verplanken, B., Hazenberg, P. T., & Palenéwen, G. R. (1992). Need for cognition and external information search effort. Journal of Research in Personality, 26(2), 128–136. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(92)90049-A
von Stumm, S. & Ackerman, P. L. (2013). Investment and intellect: A review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 841–869. doi:10.1037/a0030746
Weißgerber, C. S., Reinhard, M.-A., & Schindler, S. (2018). Learning the hard way: Need for cognition influences attitudes towards and self-reported use of desirable learning difficulties. Educational Psychology, 38(2), 176–202. doi:10.1080/01443410.2017.1387644